BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

188 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29450793)

  • 1. The Quality of Response Time Data Inference: A Blinded, Collaborative Assessment of the Validity of Cognitive Models.
    Dutilh G; Annis J; Brown SD; Cassey P; Evans NJ; Grasman RPPP; Hawkins GE; Heathcote A; Holmes WR; Krypotos AM; Kupitz CN; Leite FP; Lerche V; Lin YS; Logan GD; Palmeri TJ; Starns JJ; Trueblood JS; van Maanen L; van Ravenzwaaij D; Vandekerckhove J; Visser I; Voss A; White CN; Wiecki TV; Rieskamp J; Donkin C
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2019 Aug; 26(4):1051-1069. PubMed ID: 29450793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Assessing the practical differences between model selection methods in inferences about choice response time tasks.
    Evans NJ
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2019 Aug; 26(4):1070-1098. PubMed ID: 30783896
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Unfalsifiability and mutual translatability of major modeling schemes for choice reaction time.
    Jones M; Dzhafarov EN
    Psychol Rev; 2014 Jan; 121(1):1-32. PubMed ID: 24079307
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: different models for response time with different conclusions about psychological mechanisms?
    Heathcote A; Hayes B
    Can J Exp Psychol; 2012 Jun; 66(2):125-36. PubMed ID: 22686161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Psychological interpretation of the ex-Gaussian and shifted Wald parameters: a diffusion model analysis.
    Matzke D; Wagenmakers EJ
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2009 Oct; 16(5):798-817. PubMed ID: 19815782
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Bayes factors for the linear ballistic accumulator model of decision-making.
    Evans NJ; Brown SD
    Behav Res Methods; 2018 Apr; 50(2):589-603. PubMed ID: 28455795
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Testing the validity of conflict drift-diffusion models for use in estimating cognitive processes: A parameter-recovery study.
    White CN; Servant M; Logan GD
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2018 Feb; 25(1):286-301. PubMed ID: 28357629
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Dynamic models of choice.
    Heathcote A; Lin YS; Reynolds A; Strickland L; Gretton M; Matzke D
    Behav Res Methods; 2019 Apr; 51(2):961-985. PubMed ID: 29959755
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Bayesian statistical approaches to evaluating cognitive models.
    Annis J; Palmeri TJ
    Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci; 2018 Mar; 9(2):. PubMed ID: 29193776
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. A cognitive model-based approach to testing mechanistic explanations for neuropsychological decrements during tobacco abstinence.
    Weigard A; Huang-Pollock C; Heathcote A; Hawk L; Schlienz NJ
    Psychopharmacology (Berl); 2018 Nov; 235(11):3115-3124. PubMed ID: 30182252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Assessing the validity of multinomial models using extraneous variables: an application to prospective memory.
    Rummel J; Boywitt CD; Meiser T
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2011 Nov; 64(11):2194-210. PubMed ID: 21736435
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. An alternative approach to analysis of mental states in experimental social cognition research.
    Lench HC; Taylor AB; Bench SW
    Behav Res Methods; 2014 Mar; 46(1):215-28. PubMed ID: 23709165
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Speed-accuracy manipulations and diffusion modeling: Lack of discriminant validity of the manipulation or of the parameter estimates?
    Lerche V; Voss A
    Behav Res Methods; 2018 Dec; 50(6):2568-2585. PubMed ID: 29542062
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The trade-off between accuracy and precision in latent variable models of mediation processes.
    Ledgerwood A; Shrout PE
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2011 Dec; 101(6):1174-88. PubMed ID: 21806305
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Integrating cognitive process and descriptive models of attitudes and preferences.
    Hawkins GE; Marley AA; Heathcote A; Flynn TN; Louviere JJ; Brown SD
    Cogn Sci; 2014; 38(4):701-35. PubMed ID: 24124986
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Dynamic cognitive models of intertemporal choice.
    Dai J; Pleskac TJ; Pachur T
    Cogn Psychol; 2018 Aug; 104():29-56. PubMed ID: 29587183
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Evidence accumulation in a complex task: Making choices about concurrent multiattribute stimuli under time pressure.
    Palada H; Neal A; Vuckovic A; Martin R; Samuels K; Heathcote A
    J Exp Psychol Appl; 2016 Mar; 22(1):1-23. PubMed ID: 26844369
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Modeling accuracy, response time, and bias in continuous orientation judgments.
    Kvam PD
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2019 Mar; 45(3):301-318. PubMed ID: 30714760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Approximating Bayesian Inference through Model Simulation.
    Turner BM; Van Zandt T
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2018 Sep; 22(9):826-840. PubMed ID: 30093313
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Cognitive models of physicians' legal standard and personal judgments of competency in patients with Alzheimer's disease.
    Earnst KS; Marson DC; Harrell LE
    J Am Geriatr Soc; 2000 Aug; 48(8):919-27. PubMed ID: 10968295
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.