These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

165 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29454819)

  • 1. Some inferences still take time: Prosody, predictability, and the speed of scalar implicatures.
    Huang YT; Snedeker J
    Cogn Psychol; 2018 May; 102():105-126. PubMed ID: 29454819
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Are scalar implicatures automatically processed and different for each individual? A mismatch negativity (MMN) study.
    Zhao M; Liu T; Chen G; Chen F
    Brain Res; 2015 Mar; 1599():137-49. PubMed ID: 25542387
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface.
    Papafragou A; Musolino J
    Cognition; 2003 Jan; 86(3):253-82. PubMed ID: 12485740
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Processing inferences at the semantics/pragmatics frontier: disjunctions and free choice.
    Chemla E; Bott L
    Cognition; 2014 Mar; 130(3):380-96. PubMed ID: 24389313
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. What second-language speakers can tell us about pragmatic processing.
    Khorsheed A; Md Rashid S; Nimehchisalem V; Geok Imm L; Price J; Ronderos CR
    PLoS One; 2022; 17(2):e0263724. PubMed ID: 35180247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. When some is not every: dissociating scalar implicature generation and mismatch.
    Shetreet E; Chierchia G; Gaab N
    Hum Brain Mapp; 2014 Apr; 35(4):1503-14. PubMed ID: 23568365
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. On children's variable success with scalar inferences: Insights from disjunction in the scope of a universal quantifier.
    Pagliarini E; Bill C; Romoli J; Tieu L; Crain S
    Cognition; 2018 Sep; 178():178-192. PubMed ID: 29879554
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The neural integration of speaker and message.
    Van Berkum JJ; van den Brink D; Tesink CM; Kos M; Hagoort P
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2008 Apr; 20(4):580-91. PubMed ID: 18052777
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Online pragmatic interpretations of scalar adjectives are affected by perceived speaker reliability.
    Gardner B; Dix S; Lawrence R; Morgan C; Sullivan A; Kurumada C
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(2):e0245130. PubMed ID: 33606683
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Semantic meaning and pragmatic interpretation in 5-year-olds: evidence from real-time spoken language comprehension.
    Huang YT; Snedeker J
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1723-39. PubMed ID: 19899927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Availability of Alternatives and the Processing of Scalar Implicatures: A Visual World Eye-Tracking Study.
    Degen J; Tanenhaus MK
    Cogn Sci; 2016 Jan; 40(1):172-201. PubMed ID: 25807866
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Untrained speakers' use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation and listeners' interpretations.
    Tree JE; Meijer PJ
    Psychol Res; 2000; 63(1):1-13. PubMed ID: 10743382
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Neural modelling of the semantic predictability gain under challenging listening conditions.
    Rysop AU; Schmitt LM; Obleser J; Hartwigsen G
    Hum Brain Mapp; 2021 Jan; 42(1):110-127. PubMed ID: 32959939
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review.
    Pickering MJ; Gambi C
    Psychol Bull; 2018 Oct; 144(10):1002-1044. PubMed ID: 29952584
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Guess who? Children use prosody to infer intended listeners.
    Varghese AL; Nilsen ES
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2016 Jun; 34(2):306-12. PubMed ID: 26871544
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The role of alternative salience in the derivation of scalar implicatures.
    Rees A; Bott L
    Cognition; 2018 Jul; 176():1-14. PubMed ID: 29529396
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Why wait for the verb? Turkish speaking children use case markers for incremental language comprehension.
    Özge D; Küntay A; Snedeker J
    Cognition; 2019 Feb; 183():152-180. PubMed ID: 30468979
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Using prosody to infer discourse prominence in cochlear-implant users and normal-hearing listeners.
    Huang YT; Newman RS; Catalano A; Goupell MJ
    Cognition; 2017 Sep; 166():184-200. PubMed ID: 28578222
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Are generalised scalar implicatures generated by default? An on-line investigation into the role of context in generating pragmatic inferences.
    Breheny R; Katsos N; Williams J
    Cognition; 2006 Jul; 100(3):434-63. PubMed ID: 16115617
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The Trouble With Quantifiers: Exploring Children's Deficits in Scalar Implicature.
    Horowitz AC; Schneider RM; Frank MC
    Child Dev; 2018 Nov; 89(6):e572-e593. PubMed ID: 29285759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.