BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

135 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29514559)

  • 1. Reversed Depth in Anticorrelated Random-Dot Stereograms and the Central-Peripheral Difference in Visual Inference.
    Zhaoping L; Ackermann J
    Perception; 2018 Jan; ():301006618758571. PubMed ID: 29514559
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Contrast-reversed binocular dot-pairs in random-dot stereograms for depth perception in central visual field: Probing the dynamics of feedforward-feedback processes in visual inference.
    Zhaoping L
    Vision Res; 2021 Sep; 186():124-139. PubMed ID: 34091397
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Disparity detection in anticorrelated stereograms.
    Cumming BG; Shapiro SE; Parker AJ
    Perception; 1998; 27(11):1367-77. PubMed ID: 10505181
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception.
    Cumming BG; Parker AJ
    Nature; 1997 Sep; 389(6648):280-3. PubMed ID: 9305841
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. A Single Mechanism Can Account for Human Perception of Depth in Mixed Correlation Random Dot Stereograms.
    Henriksen S; Cumming BG; Read JC
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2016 May; 12(5):e1004906. PubMed ID: 27196696
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Rejection of false matches for binocular correspondence in macaque visual cortical area V4.
    Tanabe S; Umeda K; Fujita I
    J Neurosci; 2004 Sep; 24(37):8170-80. PubMed ID: 15371518
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Disparity Sensitivity and Binocular Integration in Mouse Visual Cortex Areas.
    La Chioma A; Bonhoeffer T; Hübener M
    J Neurosci; 2020 Nov; 40(46):8883-8899. PubMed ID: 33051348
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. EEG response in humans for frequency-tagged anticorrelated random-dot stereograms: Increased coherency and alpha oscillations.
    Derzsi Z
    Front Neurosci; 2022; 16():909225. PubMed ID: 35968391
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Robust natural depth for anticorrelated random dot stereogram for edge stimuli, but minimal reversed depth for embedded circular stimuli, irrespective of eccentricity.
    Hibbard PB; Asher JM
    PLoS One; 2022; 17(9):e0274566. PubMed ID: 36137132
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. A relative frame of reference underlies reversed depth perception in anticorrelated random-dot stereograms.
    Aoki SC; Shiozaki HM; Fujita I
    J Vis; 2017 Oct; 17(12):17. PubMed ID: 29071354
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Depth in anticorrelated stereograms: effects of spatial density and interocular delay.
    Cogan AI; Lomakin AJ; Rossi AF
    Vision Res; 1993 Sep; 33(14):1959-75. PubMed ID: 8249313
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Neurons in Striate Cortex Signal Disparity in Half-Matched Random-Dot Stereograms.
    Henriksen S; Read JC; Cumming BG
    J Neurosci; 2016 Aug; 36(34):8967-76. PubMed ID: 27559177
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Disparity in Context: Understanding how monocular image content interacts with disparity processing in human visual cortex.
    Duan Y; Thatte J; Yaklovleva A; Norcia AM
    Neuroimage; 2021 Aug; 237():118139. PubMed ID: 33964460
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Cross-matching: a modified cross-correlation underlying threshold energy model and match-based depth perception.
    Doi T; Fujita I
    Front Comput Neurosci; 2014; 8():127. PubMed ID: 25360107
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Can random-dot stereograms serve as a model for the perception of depth in relation to real three-dimensional objects?
    Bishop PO
    Vision Res; 1996 May; 36(10):1473-7. PubMed ID: 8762764
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Learning to see random-dot stereograms.
    O'Toole AJ; Kersten DJ
    Perception; 1992; 21(2):227-43. PubMed ID: 1513672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Unconscious adaptation: a new illusion of depth induced by stimulus features without depth.
    Hayashi R; Miyawaki Y; Maeda T; Tachi S
    Vision Res; 2003 Dec; 43(26):2773-82. PubMed ID: 14568094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The Flip Tilt Illusion: Visible in Peripheral Vision as Predicted by the Central-Peripheral Dichotomy.
    Zhaoping L
    Iperception; 2020; 11(4):2041669520938408. PubMed ID: 32782769
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Depth perception not found in human observers for static or dynamic anti-correlated random dot stereograms.
    Hibbard PB; Scott-Brown KC; Haigh EC; Adrain M
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(1):e84087. PubMed ID: 24416195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Interhemispherical comparisons in the processing of contour and random texture sinewave stereograms.
    Shipley T; Garfinkel R; van Houten P
    Doc Ophthalmol; 1984 Nov; 58(3):269-305. PubMed ID: 6518980
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.