These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

106 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29624833)

  • 1. Infants' visual sustained attention is higher during joint play than solo play: is this due to increased endogenous attention control or exogenous stimulus capture?
    Wass SV; Clackson K; Georgieva SD; Brightman L; Nutbrown R; Leong V
    Dev Sci; 2018 Nov; 21(6):e12667. PubMed ID: 29624833
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Parental neural responsivity to infants' visual attention: How mature brains influence immature brains during social interaction.
    Wass SV; Noreika V; Georgieva S; Clackson K; Brightman L; Nutbrown R; Covarrubias LS; Leong V
    PLoS Biol; 2018 Dec; 16(12):e2006328. PubMed ID: 30543622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Variability in infant social responsiveness: Age and situational differences in attention-following.
    Tang Y; Triesch J; Deák GO
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2023 Oct; 63():101283. PubMed ID: 37586147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Infants' online perception of give-and-take interactions.
    Elsner C; Bakker M; Rohlfing K; Gredebäck G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Oct; 126(100):280-94. PubMed ID: 24973626
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Linking Joint Attention with Hand-Eye Coordination - A Sensorimotor Approach to Understanding Child-Parent Social Interaction.
    Yu C; Smith LB
    Cogsci; 2015 Jul; 2015():2763-2768. PubMed ID: 29226280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Early development of saliency-driven attention through object manipulation.
    Sun L; Francis DJ; Nagai Y; Yoshida H
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2024 Mar; 243():104124. PubMed ID: 38232506
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Touchscreens can promote infant object-interlocutor reference switching.
    Hudspeth KM; Lewis C
    Infant Behav Dev; 2024 Mar; 74():101914. PubMed ID: 38065036
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. What is moving where? Infants' visual attention to dynamic objects may assist with processing of spatial relations.
    Choi J; Park Y
    Front Psychol; 2023; 14():1261201. PubMed ID: 38303777
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Do Helpful Mothers Help? Effects of Maternal Scaffolding and Infant Engagement on Cognitive Performance.
    Clackson K; Wass S; Georgieva S; Brightman L; Nutbrown R; Almond H; Bieluczyk J; Carro G; Rigby Dames B; Leong V
    Front Psychol; 2019; 10():2661. PubMed ID: 31849773
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The slow emergence of gaze- and point-following: A longitudinal study of infants from 4 to 12 months.
    Tang Y; Gonzalez MR; Deák GO
    Dev Sci; 2024 May; 27(3):e13457. PubMed ID: 37941084
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Infant sustained attention but not joint attention to objects at 9 months predicts vocabulary at 12 and 15 months.
    Yu C; Suanda SH; Smith LB
    Dev Sci; 2019 Jan; 22(1):e12735. PubMed ID: 30255968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Observing Third-Party Attentional Relationships Affects Infants' Gaze Following: An Eye-Tracking Study.
    Meng X; Uto Y; Hashiya K
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():2065. PubMed ID: 28149284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Controlling the input: How one-year-old infants sustain visual attention.
    Mendez AH; Yu C; Smith LB
    Dev Sci; 2024 Mar; 27(2):e13445. PubMed ID: 37665124
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Human and monkey infant attention to dynamic social and nonsocial stimuli.
    Maylott SE; Paukner A; Ahn YA; Simpson EA
    Dev Psychobiol; 2020 Sep; 62(6):841-857. PubMed ID: 32424813
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Parent-infant affect synchrony during social and solo play.
    Kidby S; Neale D; Wass S; Leong V
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2023 Apr; 378(1875):20210482. PubMed ID: 36871594
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Direct and Observed Joint Attention Modulate 9-Month-Old Infants' Object Encoding.
    Thiele M; Kalinke S; Michel C; Haun DBM
    Open Mind (Camb); 2023; 7():917-946. PubMed ID: 38053630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Not just playing around: infants' behaviors with objects reflect ability, constraints, and object properties.
    Lobo MA; Kokkoni E; de Campos AC; Galloway JC
    Infant Behav Dev; 2014 Aug; 37(3):334-51. PubMed ID: 24879412
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Reaching experience increases face preference in 3-month-old infants.
    Libertus K; Needham A
    Dev Sci; 2011 Nov; 14(6):1355-64. PubMed ID: 22010895
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Infants' Selectively Pay Attention to the Information They Receive from a Native Speaker of Their Language.
    Marno H; Guellai B; Vidal Y; Franzoi J; Nespor M; Mehler J
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():1150. PubMed ID: 27536263
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Comparing methods for measuring peak look duration: are individual differences observed on screen-based tasks also found in more ecologically valid contexts?
    Wass SV
    Infant Behav Dev; 2014 Aug; 37(3):315-25. PubMed ID: 24905901
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.