These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

204 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29850924)

  • 1. Free choice tasks as random generation tasks: an investigation through working memory manipulations.
    Naefgen C; Janczyk M
    Exp Brain Res; 2018 Aug; 236(8):2263-2275. PubMed ID: 29850924
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Willed action and attention to the selection of action.
    Lau HC; Rogers RD; Ramnani N; Passingham RE
    Neuroimage; 2004 Apr; 21(4):1407-15. PubMed ID: 15050566
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Why free choices take longer than forced choices: evidence from response threshold manipulations.
    Naefgen C; Dambacher M; Janczyk M
    Psychol Res; 2018 Nov; 82(6):1039-1052. PubMed ID: 28776264
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Stimulus-response links and the backward crosstalk effect - A comparison of forced- and free-choice tasks.
    Naefgen C; Caissie AF; Janczyk M
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2017 Jun; 177():23-29. PubMed ID: 28448839
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Smaller backward crosstalk effects for free choice tasks are not the result of immediate conflict adaptation.
    Naefgen C; Janczyk M
    Cogn Process; 2019 Feb; 20(1):73-85. PubMed ID: 30306368
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Method matters: systematic effects of testing procedure on visual working memory sensitivity.
    Makovski T; Watson LM; Koutstaal W; Jiang YV
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2010 Nov; 36(6):1466-79. PubMed ID: 20854011
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Independent operation of implicit working memory under cognitive load.
    Ji E; Lee KM; Kim MS
    Conscious Cogn; 2017 Oct; 55():214-222. PubMed ID: 28892738
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Dissociating working memory updating and automatic updating: The reference-back paradigm.
    Rac-Lubashevsky R; Kessler Y
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2016 Jun; 42(6):951-969. PubMed ID: 26618910
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing.
    Janczyk M; Dambacher M; Bieleke M; Gollwitzer PM
    Psychol Res; 2015 Mar; 79(2):206-20. PubMed ID: 24619532
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Dissociating decision strategies in free-choice tasks - A mouse tracking analysis.
    Vogel D; Scherbaum S; Janczyk M
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2018 Oct; 190():65-71. PubMed ID: 30015137
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. No differences in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks.
    Janczyk M; Nolden S; Jolicoeur P
    Psychol Res; 2015 May; 79(3):463-77. PubMed ID: 24947757
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The functional role of working memory in the (re-)planning and execution of grasping movements.
    Spiegel MA; Koester D; Schack T
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2013 Oct; 39(5):1326-39. PubMed ID: 23339349
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Visual working memory load does not affect the overall stimulus processing time in visual search.
    Xin K; Li Z
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2020 Mar; 73(3):330-343. PubMed ID: 31552806
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. An energy-efficient intrinsic functional organization of human working memory: A resting-state functional connectivity study.
    Liu H; Yu H; Li Y; Qin W; Xu L; Yu C; Liang M
    Behav Brain Res; 2017 Jan; 316():66-73. PubMed ID: 27569182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Dissociating the role of the caudate nucleus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the monitoring of events within human working memory.
    Provost JS; Petrides M; Monchi O
    Eur J Neurosci; 2010 Sep; 32(5):873-80. PubMed ID: 20722715
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Event-related theta oscillations during working memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls.
    Schmiedt C; Brand A; Hildebrandt H; Basar-Eroglu C
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2005 Dec; 25(3):936-47. PubMed ID: 16289526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Interference with performance of a response selection task that has no working memory component: an rTMS comparison of the dorsolateral prefrontal and medial frontal cortex.
    Hadland KA; Rushworth MF; Passingham RE; Jahanshahi M; Rothwell JC
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2001 Nov; 13(8):1097-108. PubMed ID: 11784448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Response selection in dual task paradigms: observations from random generation tasks.
    Dirnberger G; Jahanshahi M
    Exp Brain Res; 2010 Mar; 201(3):535-48. PubMed ID: 20232508
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Decomposing the n-back task: An individual differences study using the reference-back paradigm.
    Rac-Lubashevsky R; Kessler Y
    Neuropsychologia; 2016 Sep; 90():190-9. PubMed ID: 27425422
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Selective influence of working memory load on exceptionally slow reaction times.
    Shahar N; Teodorescu AR; Usher M; Pereg M; Meiran N
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2014 Oct; 143(5):1837-60. PubMed ID: 25000446
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.