These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

164 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29892412)

  • 21. Longer looking to agent with false belief at 7 but not 6 months of age.
    Hirshkowitz A; Rutherford MD
    Infant Child Dev; 2021; 30(5):e2263. PubMed ID: 35864890
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Spontaneous theory of mind in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
    Hayashi W; Arai G; Uno H; Saga N; Ikuse D; Takashio O; Iwanami A
    Psychiatry Res; 2020 Jun; 288():113025. PubMed ID: 32371312
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Principles of belief acquisition. How we read other minds.
    Pascarelli MT; Quarona D; Barchiesi G; Riva G; Butterfill SA; Sinigaglia C
    Conscious Cogn; 2024 Jan; 117():103625. PubMed ID: 38159535
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. No links between genetic variation and developing theory of mind: A preregistered replication attempt of candidate gene studies.
    Opitz T; Schuwerk T; Paulus M; Kloo D; Osterhaus C; Lesch KP; Sodian B
    Dev Sci; 2021 Sep; 24(5):e13100. PubMed ID: 33666309
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Early conversational environment enables spontaneous belief attribution in deaf children.
    Meristo M; Strid K; Hjelmquist E
    Cognition; 2016 Dec; 157():139-145. PubMed ID: 27636329
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. On the domain specificity of the mechanisms underpinning spontaneous anticipatory looks in false-belief tasks.
    Surian L; Franchin L
    Dev Sci; 2020 Nov; 23(6):e12955. PubMed ID: 32107820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Chinese preschoolers' implicit and explicit false-belief understanding.
    Wang B; Low J; Jing Z; Qinghua Q
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):123-40. PubMed ID: 22429037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. 14-Month-olds anticipate others' actions based on their belief about an object's identity.
    Buttelmann F; Kovács ÁM
    Infancy; 2019 Sep; 24(5):738-751. PubMed ID: 32677281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Fourteen- to Eighteen-Month-Old Infants Use Explicit Linguistic Information to Update an Agent's False Belief.
    Jin KS; Kim Y; Song M; Kim YJ; Lee H; Lee Y; Cha M; Song HJ
    Front Psychol; 2019; 10():2508. PubMed ID: 31824369
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Why can some implicit Theory of Mind tasks be replicated and others cannot? A test of mentalizing versus submentalizing accounts.
    Kulke L; Johannsen J; Rakoczy H
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213772. PubMed ID: 30909288
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Implicit Mentalizing Persists beyond Early Childhood and Is Profoundly Impaired in Children with Autism Spectrum Condition.
    Schuwerk T; Jarvers I; Vuori M; Sodian B
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():1696. PubMed ID: 27840620
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Eye tracking uncovered great apes' ability to anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs.
    Kano F; Krupenye C; Hirata S; Call J
    Commun Integr Biol; 2017; 10(2):e1299836. PubMed ID: 28451059
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Preschoolers' implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: relations with complex syntactical mastery.
    Low J
    Child Dev; 2010; 81(2):597-615. PubMed ID: 20438463
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Why Do Children Who Solve False Belief Tasks Begin to Find True Belief Control Tasks Difficult? A Test of Pragmatic Performance Factors in Theory of Mind Tasks.
    Schidelko LP; Huemer M; Schröder LM; Lueb AS; Perner J; Rakoczy H
    Front Psychol; 2021; 12():797246. PubMed ID: 35095682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Eighteen-month-olds' memory interference and distraction in a modified A-not-B task is not associated with their anticipatory looking in a false-belief task.
    Zmyj N; Prinz W; Daum MM
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():857. PubMed ID: 26157409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The influence of a bystander agent's beliefs on children's and adults' decision-making process.
    Buttelmann F; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jan; 153():126-139. PubMed ID: 27741442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Perceptual Access Reasoning (PAR) in Developing a Representational Theory of Mind.
    Fabricius WV; Gonzales CR; Pesch A; Weimer AA; Pugliese J; Carroll K; Bolnick RR; Kupfer AS; Eisenberg N; Spinrad TL
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 86(3):7-154. PubMed ID: 34580875
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Memory and inferential processes in false-belief tasks: An investigation of the unexpected-contents paradigm.
    Rubio-Fernández P
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Jan; 177():297-312. PubMed ID: 30269985
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Reliability and generalizability of an acted-out false belief task in 3-year-olds.
    Dörrenberg S; Wenzel L; Proft M; Rakoczy H; Liszkowski U
    Infant Behav Dev; 2019 Feb; 54():13-21. PubMed ID: 30513405
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.