These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

195 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29980941)

  • 21. Copy me or copy you? The effect of prior experience on social learning.
    Wood LA; Kendal RL; Flynn EG
    Cognition; 2013 May; 127(2):203-13. PubMed ID: 23454793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. When does it pay to follow the crowd? Children optimize imitation of causally irrelevant actions performed by a majority.
    Evans CL; Burdett ERR; Murray K; Carpenter M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2021 Dec; 212():105229. PubMed ID: 34284228
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Young children contrast their behavior to that of out-group members.
    Oostenbroek J; Over H
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Nov; 139():234-41. PubMed ID: 26105978
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions.
    McGuigan N
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 May; 126(2):150-60. PubMed ID: 21942570
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. The Rationality of (Over)imitation.
    Keupp S; Behne T; Rakoczy H
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2018 Nov; 13(6):678-687. PubMed ID: 30301424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. What's Special about Human Imitation? A Comparison with Enculturated Apes.
    Subiaul F
    Behav Sci (Basel); 2016 Jul; 6(3):. PubMed ID: 27399786
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Social Learning in the Real-World: 'Over-Imitation' Occurs in Both Children and Adults Unaware of Participation in an Experiment and Independently of Social Interaction.
    Whiten A; Allan G; Devlin S; Kseib N; Raw N; McGuigan N
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(7):e0159920. PubMed ID: 27466806
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Emulation and "overemulation" in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds.
    McGuigan N; Whiten A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Dec; 104(4):367-81. PubMed ID: 19683722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior.
    Over H; Carpenter M
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 May; 126(2):182-92. PubMed ID: 21767011
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly "optimal-" rather than "over-" imitators.
    Evans CL; Laland KN; Carpenter M; Kendal RL
    Dev Sci; 2018 Sep; 21(5):e12637. PubMed ID: 29250871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. "Model age-based" and "copy when uncertain" biases in children's social learning of a novel task.
    Wood LA; Harrison RA; Lucas AJ; McGuigan N; Burdett ERR; Whiten A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Oct; 150():272-284. PubMed ID: 27371768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions.
    Tennie C; Call J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(8):e41548. PubMed ID: 22905102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Do as I … Did! Long-term memory of imitative actions in dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Fugazza C; Pogány Á; Miklósi Á
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):263-9. PubMed ID: 26498155
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Social learning of goal-directed actions in dogs (Canis familiaris): Imitation or emulation?
    Fugazza C; Petro E; Miklósi Á; Pogány Á
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 May; 133(2):244-251. PubMed ID: 30407032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees.
    Tomasello M; Savage-Rumbaugh S; Kruger AC
    Child Dev; 1993 Dec; 64(6):1688-705. PubMed ID: 8112113
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Cross-cultural variation in how much, but not whether, children overimitate.
    Stengelin R; Hepach R; Haun DBM
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 May; 193():104796. PubMed ID: 31987592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Deferred imitation and declarative memory in domestic dogs.
    Fugazza C; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):237-47. PubMed ID: 23856708
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Carry-over effects of tool functionality and previous unsuccessfulness increase overimitation in children.
    Frick A; Schleihauf H; Satchell LP; Gruber T
    R Soc Open Sci; 2021 Jul; 8(7):201373. PubMed ID: 34295509
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Two-year-old children copy more reliably and more often than nonhuman great apes in multiple observational learning tasks.
    Tennie C; Greve K; Gretscher H; Call J
    Primates; 2010 Oct; 51(4):337-51. PubMed ID: 20686814
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The ethological analysis of imitation.
    Miklósi A
    Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc; 1999 Aug; 74(3):347-74. PubMed ID: 10466254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.