BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

190 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 30112598)

  • 1. Dogs do not demonstrate a human-like bias to defer to communicative cues.
    Johnston AM; Huang Y; Santos LR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):449-461. PubMed ID: 30112598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Visual perspective taking by dogs (Canis familiaris) in a Guesser-Knower task: evidence for a canine theory of mind?
    Maginnity ME; Grace RC
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Nov; 17(6):1375-92. PubMed ID: 24950722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. How dogs know when communication is intended for them.
    Kaminski J; Schulz L; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2012 Mar; 15(2):222-32. PubMed ID: 22356178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. What experimental experience affects dogs' comprehension of human communicative actions?
    Hauser MD; Comins JA; Pytka LM; Cahill DP; Velez-Calderon S
    Behav Processes; 2011 Jan; 86(1):7-20. PubMed ID: 20696218
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Reassessing shelter dogs' use of human communicative cues in the standard object-choice task.
    Osborne T; Mulcahy NJ
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213166. PubMed ID: 30845205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Two-year-old children but not domestic dogs understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze.
    Moore R; Mueller B; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2015 Mar; 18(2):232-42. PubMed ID: 25041186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible.
    Dwyer C; Cole MR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):442-448. PubMed ID: 30251105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.
    Kirchhofer KC; Zimmermann F; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(2):e30913. PubMed ID: 22347411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Adapting to the human world: dogs' responsiveness to our social cues.
    Reid PJ
    Behav Processes; 2009 Mar; 80(3):325-33. PubMed ID: 19056474
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Selective responding to human ostensive communication is an early developing capacity of domestic dogs.
    Byosiere SE; Mundry R; Range F; Virányi Z
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13361. PubMed ID: 36545915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Sensitivity to communicative and non-communicative gestures in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder: saccadic and pupillary responses.
    Aldaqre I; Schuwerk T; Daum MM; Sodian B; Paulus M
    Exp Brain Res; 2016 Sep; 234(9):2515-27. PubMed ID: 27119361
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task?
    Kupán K; Miklósi Á; Gergely G; Topál J
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Mar; 14(2):259-68. PubMed ID: 21140185
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans.
    Miklósi A; Pongrácz P; Lakatos G; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 May; 119(2):179-86. PubMed ID: 15982161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Adjustment in the point-following behaviour of free-ranging dogs - roles of social petting and informative-deceptive nature of cues.
    Bhattacharjee D; Bhadra A
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Jun; 25(3):571-579. PubMed ID: 34743236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Testing the social dog hypothesis: are dogs also more skilled than chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks?
    Wobber V; Hare B
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):423-8. PubMed ID: 19376207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Dogs demonstrate perspective taking based on geometrical gaze following in a Guesser-Knower task.
    Catala A; Mang B; Wallis L; Huber L
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Jul; 20(4):581-589. PubMed ID: 28342160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners.
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Barrera G; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):402-8. PubMed ID: 19520240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Communicative intent modulates production and comprehension of actions and gestures: A Kinect study.
    Trujillo JP; Simanova I; Bekkering H; Özyürek A
    Cognition; 2018 Nov; 180():38-51. PubMed ID: 29981967
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Canine perspective-taking.
    Huber L; Lonardo L
    Anim Cogn; 2023 Jan; 26(1):275-298. PubMed ID: 36629935
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.