These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

162 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 30120727)

  • 41. Do dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) make counterproductive choices because they are sensitive to human ostensive cues?
    Marshall-Pescini S; Passalacqua C; Miletto Petrazzini ME; Valsecchi P; Prato-Previde E
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(4):e35437. PubMed ID: 22558150
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Can squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) learn self-control? A study using food array selection tests and reverse-reward contingency.
    Anderson JR; Awazu S; Fujita K
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2000 Jan; 26(1):87-97. PubMed ID: 10650546
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Colour versus quantity as cues in reverse-reward-competent squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus).
    Anderson JR; Awazu S; Fujita K
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Apr; 62(4):673-80. PubMed ID: 18720276
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. My owner, right or wrong: the effect of familiarity on the domestic dog's behavior in a food-choice task.
    Cook A; Arter J; Jacobs LF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):461-70. PubMed ID: 23995771
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. An investigation on the olfactory capabilities of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).
    Ferrando E; Dahl CD
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Dec; 25(6):1567-1577. PubMed ID: 35689114
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Awake canine fMRI predicts dogs' preference for praise vs food.
    Cook PF; Prichard A; Spivak M; Berns GS
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2016 Dec; 11(12):1853-1862. PubMed ID: 27521302
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. When is it worth waiting for? Food quantity, but not food quality, affects delay tolerance in tufted capuchin monkeys.
    De Petrillo F; Gori E; Micucci A; Ponsi G; Paglieri F; Addessi E
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Sep; 18(5):1019-29. PubMed ID: 25894673
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Relational concept learning in domestic dogs: Performance on a two-choice size discrimination task generalises to novel stimuli.
    Byosiere SE; Feng LC; Chouinard PA; Howell TJ; Bennett PC
    Behav Processes; 2017 Dec; 145():93-101. PubMed ID: 29056526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Comparison of the cognitive palatability assessment protocol and the two-pan test for use in assessing palatability of two similar foods in dogs.
    Araujo JA; Studzinski CM; Larson BT; Milgram NW
    Am J Vet Res; 2004 Nov; 65(11):1490-6. PubMed ID: 15566086
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Using judgment bias test in pet and shelter dogs (Canis familiaris): Methodological and statistical caveats.
    Burani C; Barnard S; Wells D; Pelosi A; Valsecchi P
    PLoS One; 2020; 15(10):e0241344. PubMed ID: 33108399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Can dogs (Canis familiaris) detect human deception?
    Petter M; Musolino E; Roberts WA; Cole M
    Behav Processes; 2009 Oct; 82(2):109-18. PubMed ID: 19686949
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food.
    Udell MA; Giglio RF; Wynne CD
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 Feb; 122(1):84-93. PubMed ID: 18298285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Impact of stimulus format and reward value on quantity discrimination in capuchin and squirrel monkeys.
    Gazes RP; Billas AR; Schmitt V
    Learn Behav; 2018 Mar; 46(1):89-100. PubMed ID: 28840526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task.
    Bräuer J; Bös M; Call J; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Mar; 16(2):273-85. PubMed ID: 23090682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures.
    Feuerbacher EN; Wynne CD
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():47-59. PubMed ID: 25173617
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Inverse sex effects on performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in a repeated problem-solving task.
    Duranton C; Rödel HG; Bedossa T; Belkhir S
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Feb; 129(1):84-7. PubMed ID: 25181448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Dogs learn to solve the support problem based on perceptual cues.
    Müller CA; Riemer S; Virányi Z; Huber L; Range F
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Sep; 17(5):1071-80. PubMed ID: 24590804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Mechanisms of impulsive choice: III. The role of reward processes.
    Marshall AT; Kirkpatrick K
    Behav Processes; 2016 Feb; 123():134-48. PubMed ID: 26506254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible.
    Dwyer C; Cole MR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):442-448. PubMed ID: 30251105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.