These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

218 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 30489120)

  • 21. Worse than measurement error: Consequences of inappropriate latent variable measurement models.
    Rhemtulla M; van Bork R; Borsboom D
    Psychol Methods; 2020 Feb; 25(1):30-45. PubMed ID: 31169371
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Examining reproducibility in psychology: A hybrid method for combining a statistically significant original study and a replication.
    van Aert RCM; van Assen MALM
    Behav Res Methods; 2018 Aug; 50(4):1515-1539. PubMed ID: 28936638
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Constraints on Generality (COG): A Proposed Addition to All Empirical Papers.
    Simons DJ; Shoda Y; Lindsay DS
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2017 Nov; 12(6):1123-1128. PubMed ID: 28853993
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Challenges and suggestions for defining replication "success" when effects may be heterogeneous: Comment on Hedges and Schauer (2019).
    Mathur MB; VanderWeele TJ
    Psychol Methods; 2019 Oct; 24(5):571-575. PubMed ID: 31580141
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Graphical Descriptives: A Way to Improve Data Transparency and Methodological Rigor in Psychology.
    Tay L; Parrigon S; Huang Q; LeBreton JM
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2016 Sep; 11(5):692-701. PubMed ID: 27694464
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Replicator degrees of freedom allow publication of misleading failures to replicate.
    Bryan CJ; Yeager DS; O'Brien JM
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2019 Dec; 116(51):25535-25545. PubMed ID: 31767750
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Moving beyond unwise replication practices: The case of romantic motivation.
    Sundie JM; Beal DJ; Neuberg SL; Kenrick DT
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2019 Apr; 148(4):e1-e11. PubMed ID: 30973258
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Shifting Minds: A Quantitative Reappraisal of Cognitive-Intervention Research.
    Moreau D
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2021 Jan; 16(1):148-160. PubMed ID: 33021890
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Health Psychology adopts Transparency and Openness Promotion (TOP) Guidelines.
    Freedland KE
    Health Psychol; 2021 Apr; 40(4):227-229. PubMed ID: 33856829
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Comparing meta-analyses and preregistered multiple-laboratory replication projects.
    Kvarven A; Strømland E; Johannesson M
    Nat Hum Behav; 2020 Apr; 4(4):423-434. PubMed ID: 31873200
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Reconsideration of the type I error rate for psychological science in the era of replication.
    Carlin MT; Costello MS; Flansburg MA; Darden A
    Psychol Methods; 2024 Apr; 29(2):379-387. PubMed ID: 35404627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. "What meta-analyses reveal about the replicability of psychological research": Correction to Stanley, Carter, and Doucouliagos (2018).
    Psychol Bull; 2019 Jul; 145(7):764. PubMed ID: 31192651
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. A review of applications of the Bayes factor in psychological research.
    Heck DW; Boehm U; Böing-Messing F; Bürkner PC; Derks K; Dienes Z; Fu Q; Gu X; Karimova D; Kiers HAL; Klugkist I; Kuiper RM; Lee MD; Leenders R; Leplaa HJ; Linde M; Ly A; Meijerink-Bosman M; Moerbeek M; Mulder J; Palfi B; Schönbrodt FD; Tendeiro JN; van den Bergh D; Van Lissa CJ; van Ravenzwaaij D; Vanpaemel W; Wagenmakers EJ; Williams DR; Zondervan-Zwijnenburg M; Hoijtink H
    Psychol Methods; 2023 Jun; 28(3):558-579. PubMed ID: 35298215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Statistical reporting inconsistencies in experimental philosophy.
    Colombo M; Duev G; Nuijten MB; Sprenger J
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(4):e0194360. PubMed ID: 29649220
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Transparency and Reproducibility of Meta-Analyses in Psychology: A Meta-Review.
    Polanin JR; Hennessy EA; Tsuji S
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2020 Jul; 15(4):1026-1041. PubMed ID: 32516081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Many replications do not causal inferences make: the need for critical replications to test competing explanations of nonrandomized studies.
    Larzelere RE; Cox RB; Swindle TM
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2015 May; 10(3):380-9. PubMed ID: 25987516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The seemingly quixotic pursuit of a cumulative psychological science: introduction to the special issue.
    Curran PJ
    Psychol Methods; 2009 Jun; 14(2):77-80. PubMed ID: 19485622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Psychology, Science, and Knowledge Construction: Broadening Perspectives from the Replication Crisis.
    Shrout PE; Rodgers JL
    Annu Rev Psychol; 2018 Jan; 69():487-510. PubMed ID: 29300688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. [The analysis of physicians' work: announcing the end of attempts at in vitro fertilization].
    Santiago-Delefosse M; Cahen F; Coeffin-Driol C
    Encephale; 2003; 29(4 Pt 1):293-305. PubMed ID: 14615699
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The fixed versus random effects debate and how it relates to centering in multilevel modeling.
    Hamaker EL; Muthén B
    Psychol Methods; 2020 Jun; 25(3):365-379. PubMed ID: 31613118
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.