These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

187 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 30730174)

  • 1. Associative word learning in infancy: A meta-analysis of the switch task.
    Tsui ASM; Byers-Heinlein K; Fennell CT
    Dev Psychol; 2019 May; 55(5):934-950. PubMed ID: 30730174
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Using speech sounds to guide word learning: the case of bilingual infants.
    Fennell CT; Byers-Heinlein K; Werker JF
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(5):1510-25. PubMed ID: 17883445
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Development of rapid word-object associations in relation to expressive vocabulary: Shared commonalities in infants and toddlers with and without Williams syndrome.
    Ha OR; Cashon CH; Holt NA; Mervis CB
    Dev Sci; 2020 Nov; 23(6):e12966. PubMed ID: 32196857
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The first steps in word learning are easier when the shoes fit: comparing monolingual and bilingual infants.
    Mattock K; Polka L; Rvachew S; Krehm M
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):229-43. PubMed ID: 20121879
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Difficulty in learning similar-sounding words: A developmental stage or a general property of learning?
    Pajak B; Creel SC; Levy R
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2016 Sep; 42(9):1377-99. PubMed ID: 26962959
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Using variability to guide dimensional weighting: associative mechanisms in early word learning.
    Apfelbaum KS; McMurray B
    Cogn Sci; 2011 Aug; 35(6):1105-38. PubMed ID: 21609356
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: infants' language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic.
    Byers-Heinlein K; Werker JF
    Dev Sci; 2009 Sep; 12(5):815-23. PubMed ID: 19702772
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Fourteen-month-olds' sensitivity to acoustic salience in minimal pair word learning.
    Archer SL; Curtin S
    J Child Lang; 2018 Sep; 45(5):1198-1211. PubMed ID: 29465335
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Early word learners' ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words.
    Fennell CT; Werker JF
    Lang Speech; 2003; 46(Pt 2-3):245-64. PubMed ID: 14748446
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning.
    Graf Estes K; Evans JL; Alibali MW; Saffran JR
    Psychol Sci; 2007 Mar; 18(3):254-60. PubMed ID: 17444923
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants.
    Werker JF; Cohen LB; Lloyd VL; Casasola M; Stager CL
    Dev Psychol; 1998 Nov; 34(6):1289-309. PubMed ID: 9823513
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Perceptual foundations of bilingual acquisition in infancy.
    Werker J
    Ann N Y Acad Sci; 2012 Mar; 1251():50-61. PubMed ID: 22694186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Beyond perceptual narrowing: Monolingual and bilingual infants discriminate Hindi contrasts when learning words in the second year of life.
    Singh L; Tan ARY
    Dev Psychol; 2021 Jan; 57(1):19-32. PubMed ID: 33271031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: evidence from bilingual infants.
    Byers-Heinlein K; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2013 Sep; 128(3):407-16. PubMed ID: 23774635
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Does sound structure affect word learning? An eye-tracking study of Danish learning toddlers.
    Trecca F; Bleses D; Madsen TO; Christiansen MH
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Mar; 167():180-203. PubMed ID: 29175718
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Non-adjacent dependency learning in infancy, and its link to language development.
    Frost RLA; Jessop A; Durrant S; Peter MS; Bidgood A; Pine JM; Rowland CF; Monaghan P
    Cogn Psychol; 2020 Aug; 120():101291. PubMed ID: 32197131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Infants' acceptance of phonotactically illegal word forms as object labels.
    Vukatana E; Curtin S; Graham SA
    J Child Lang; 2016 Nov; 43(6):1400-11. PubMed ID: 26671660
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Finding patterns and learning words: Infant phonotactic knowledge is associated with vocabulary size.
    Graf Estes K; Gluck SC; Grimm KJ
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Jun; 146():34-49. PubMed ID: 26905502
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Only the right noise? Effects of phonetic and visual input variability on 14-month-olds' minimal pair word learning.
    Höhle B; Fritzsche T; Meß K; Philipp M; Gafos A
    Dev Sci; 2020 Sep; 23(5):e12950. PubMed ID: 32052548
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Task-appropriate input supports word-object association in 14-month-old female infants.
    Fais L; Vatikiotis-Bateson E
    J Child Lang; 2020 Mar; 47(2):472-482. PubMed ID: 31599214
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.