These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

262 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 30958017)

  • 1. Is executive control related to working memory capacity and fluid intelligence?
    Rey-Mermet A; Gade M; Souza AS; von Bastian CC; Oberauer K
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2019 Aug; 148(8):1335-1372. PubMed ID: 30958017
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Individual differences in the executive control of attention, memory, and thought, and their associations with schizotypy.
    Kane MJ; Meier ME; Smeekens BA; Gross GM; Chun CA; Silvia PJ; Kwapil TR
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2016 Aug; 145(8):1017-1048. PubMed ID: 27454042
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Individual differences in working memory capacity, attention control, fluid intelligence, and pupillary measures of arousal.
    Robison MK; Brewer GA
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2022 Sep; 48(9):1296-1310. PubMed ID: 35389696
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Working memory and fluid intelligence: capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval.
    Unsworth N; Fukuda K; Awh E; Vogel EK
    Cogn Psychol; 2014 Jun; 71():1-26. PubMed ID: 24531497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Shortened complex span tasks can reliably measure working memory capacity.
    Foster JL; Shipstead Z; Harrison TL; Hicks KL; Redick TS; Engle RW
    Mem Cognit; 2015 Feb; 43(2):226-36. PubMed ID: 25217113
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Similarities and differences between mind-wandering and external distraction: a latent variable analysis of lapses of attention and their relation to cognitive abilities.
    Unsworth N; McMillan BD
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2014 Jul; 150():14-25. PubMed ID: 24793128
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The importance of arousal for variation in working memory capacity and attention control: A latent variable pupillometry study.
    Unsworth N; Robison MK
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2017 Dec; 43(12):1962-1987. PubMed ID: 28504528
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Working memory capacity and the antisaccade task: A microanalytic-macroanalytic investigation of individual differences in goal activation and maintenance.
    Meier ME; Smeekens BA; Silvia PJ; Kwapil TR; Kane MJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Jan; 44(1):68-84. PubMed ID: 28639800
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. What makes working memory work? A multifaceted account of the predictive power of working memory capacity.
    Robison MK; Miller AL; Wiemers EA; Ellis DM; Unsworth N; Redick TS; Brewer GA
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2024 Sep; 153(9):2193-2215. PubMed ID: 39235886
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention.
    McVay JC; Kane MJ
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2012 May; 141(2):302-320. PubMed ID: 21875246
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Drifting from slow to "D'oh!": working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors.
    McVay JC; Kane MJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2012 May; 38(3):525-549. PubMed ID: 22004270
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Fluid intelligence and executive functioning more alike than different?
    van Aken L; Kessels RP; Wingbermühle E; van der Veld WM; Egger JI
    Acta Neuropsychiatr; 2016 Feb; 28(1):31-7. PubMed ID: 26281913
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The role of mind-wandering in measurements of general aptitude.
    Mrazek MD; Smallwood J; Franklin MS; Chin JM; Baird B; Schooler JW
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2012 Nov; 141(4):788-798. PubMed ID: 22468669
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Testing a Cognitive Control Model of Human Intelligence.
    Chen Y; Spagna A; Wu T; Kim TH; Wu Q; Chen C; Wu Y; Fan J
    Sci Rep; 2019 Feb; 9(1):2898. PubMed ID: 30814663
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Carving executive control at its joints: Working memory capacity predicts stimulus-stimulus, but not stimulus-response, conflict.
    Meier ME; Kane MJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2015 Nov; 41(6):1849-72. PubMed ID: 26120774
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005).
    Kane MJ; Hambrick DZ; Conway ARA
    Psychol Bull; 2005 Jan; 131(1):66-71. PubMed ID: 15631552
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Cognitive control in context: working memory capacity and proactive control.
    Redick TS
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2014 Jan; 145():1-9. PubMed ID: 24240136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Individual differences in the delayed execution of prospective memories.
    Ball BH; Knight JB; Dewitt MR; Brewer GA
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2013; 66(12):2411-25. PubMed ID: 23679085
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: evidence for a common executive attention construct.
    McCabe DP; Roediger HL; McDaniel MA; Balota DA; Hambrick DZ
    Neuropsychology; 2010 Mar; 24(2):222-243. PubMed ID: 20230116
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span.
    Burgess GC; Gray JR; Conway ARA; Braver TS
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2011 Nov; 140(4):674-692. PubMed ID: 21787103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 14.