These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

155 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31170434)

  • 21. Can dogs use vocal intonation as a social referencing cue in an object choice task?
    Colbert-White EN; Tullis A; Andresen DR; Parker KM; Patterson KE
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Mar; 21(2):253-265. PubMed ID: 29442250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Dogs (Canis familiaris) account for body orientation but not visual barriers when responding to pointing gestures.
    MacLean EL; Krupenye C; Hare B
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Aug; 128(3):285-97. PubMed ID: 24611643
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Effect of training and familiarity on responsiveness to human cues in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Cunningham CL; Ramos MF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 May; 17(3):805-14. PubMed ID: 24318516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Primates' Socio-Cognitive Abilities: What Kind of Comparisons Makes Sense?
    Byrnit JT
    Integr Psychol Behav Sci; 2015 Sep; 49(3):485-511. PubMed ID: 26016621
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Seeing the experimenter influences the response to pointing cues in long-tailed macaques.
    Schmitt V; Schloegl C; Fischer J
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(3):e91348. PubMed ID: 24646501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Perseveration on a reversal-learning task correlates with rates of self-directed behavior in nonhuman primates.
    Judge PG; Evans DW; Schroepfer KK; Gross AC
    Behav Brain Res; 2011 Sep; 222(1):57-65. PubMed ID: 21419808
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Why do adult dogs (Canis familiaris) commit the A-not-B search error?
    Sümegi Z; Kis A; Miklósi Á; Topál J
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Feb; 128(1):21-30. PubMed ID: 23957741
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Development of using experimenter-given cues in infant chimpanzees: longitudinal changes in behavior and cognitive development.
    Okamoto-Barth S; Tomonaga M; Tanaka M; Matsuzawa T
    Dev Sci; 2008 Jan; 11(1):98-108. PubMed ID: 18171372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Man's underground best friend: domestic ferrets, unlike the wild forms, show evidence of dog-like social-cognitive skills.
    Hernádi A; Kis A; Turcsán B; Topál J
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(8):e43267. PubMed ID: 22905244
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs' sensitivity to human actions.
    Udell MA; Dorey NR; Wynne CD
    Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc; 2010 May; 85(2):327-45. PubMed ID: 19961472
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Nonenculturated orangutans' (Pongo pygmaeus) use of experimenter-given manual and facial cues in an object-choice task.
    Byrnit JT
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Sep; 118(3):309-15. PubMed ID: 15482058
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Piloting a new prosociality paradigm in dogs and wolves: The location choice task.
    Dale R; Despraz MN; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F
    Behav Processes; 2019 May; 162():79-85. PubMed ID: 30716384
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The effects of changes in the referential problem space of infants and toddlers (Homo sapiens): Implications for cross-species comparisons.
    Clark H; Flack ZM; Leavens DA
    J Comp Psychol; 2020 Aug; 134(3):330-340. PubMed ID: 32091227
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Learning and owner-stranger effects on interspecific communication in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 May; 81(1):44-9. PubMed ID: 19429195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Pattern of nipple use by puppies: A comparison of the dingo (Canis dingo) and the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
    Hudson R; Rödel HG; Elizalde MT; Arteaga L; Kennedy GA; Smith BP
    J Comp Psychol; 2016 Aug; 130(3):269-77. PubMed ID: 27135263
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Sea lions' (Zalophus californianus) use of human pointing gestures as referential cues.
    Malassis R; Delfour F
    Learn Behav; 2015 Jun; 43(2):101-12. PubMed ID: 25678395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible.
    Dwyer C; Cole MR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):442-448. PubMed ID: 30251105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Dogs (Canis familiaris) use odor cues to show episodic-like memory for what, where, and when.
    Lo KH; Roberts WA
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 Nov; 133(4):428-441. PubMed ID: 30896229
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.