These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

124 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31271684)

  • 21. Do Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Benefit from Structural Alignment When Constructing Categories?
    Snape S; Krott A; McCleery JP
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2018 Sep; 48(9):2912-2924. PubMed ID: 29616485
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Sound before meaning: word learning in autistic disorders.
    Norbury CF; Griffiths H; Nation K
    Neuropsychologia; 2010 Dec; 48(14):4012-9. PubMed ID: 20951710
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Iconicity influences how effectively minimally verbal children with autism and ability-matched typically developing children use pictures as symbols in a search task.
    Hartley C; Allen ML
    Autism; 2015 Jul; 19(5):570-9. PubMed ID: 24916452
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. How parents introduce new words to young children: The influence of development and developmental disorders.
    Adamson LB; Bakeman R; Brandon B
    Infant Behav Dev; 2015 May; 39():148-58. PubMed ID: 25863927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Rapid word learning in 13- and 17-month-olds in a naturalistic two-word procedure: looking versus reaching measures.
    Gurteen PM; Horne PJ; Erjavec M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Jun; 109(2):201-17. PubMed ID: 21216414
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The absence of a shape bias in children's word learning.
    Cimpian A; Markman EM
    Dev Psychol; 2005 Nov; 41(6):1003-19. PubMed ID: 16351346
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Learning and Processing Abstract Words and Concepts: Insights From Typical and Atypical Development.
    Vigliocco G; Ponari M; Norbury C
    Top Cogn Sci; 2018 Jul; 10(3):533-549. PubMed ID: 29785838
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Too Much of a Good Thing: How Novelty Biases and Vocabulary Influence Known and Novel Referent Selection in 18-Month-Old Children and Associative Learning Models.
    Kucker SC; McMurray B; Samuelson LK
    Cogn Sci; 2018 May; 42 Suppl 2(Suppl 2):463-493. PubMed ID: 29630722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Referential gaze and word learning in adults with autism.
    Aldaqre I; Paulus M; Sodian B
    Autism; 2015 Nov; 19(8):944-55. PubMed ID: 25488004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Moving Word Learning to a Novel Space: A Dynamic Systems View of Referent Selection and Retention.
    Samuelson LK; Kucker SC; Spencer JP
    Cogn Sci; 2017 Feb; 41 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):52-72. PubMed ID: 27127009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Preschoolers' and adults' reliance on object shape and object function for lexical extension.
    Graham SA; Williams LD; Huber JF
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1999 Oct; 74(2):128-51. PubMed ID: 10479398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Lexical Acquisition Through Category Matching: 12-Month-Old Infants Associate Words to Visual Categories.
    Pomiechowska B; Gliga T
    Psychol Sci; 2019 Feb; 30(2):288-299. PubMed ID: 30575444
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Development of the shape bias during the second year.
    Hupp JM
    J Genet Psychol; 2015; 176(1-2):82-92. PubMed ID: 25775081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: evidence from autism.
    Preissler MA; Carey S
    Cognition; 2005 Aug; 97(1):B13-23. PubMed ID: 15925356
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Individual differences in the shape bias in preschool children with specific language impairment and typical language development: theoretical and clinical implications.
    Collisson BA; Grela B; Spaulding T; Rueckl JG; Magnuson JS
    Dev Sci; 2015 May; 18(3):373-88. PubMed ID: 25164789
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. The shape of thought.
    Markson L; Diesendruck G; Bloom P
    Dev Sci; 2008 Mar; 11(2):204-8. PubMed ID: 18333975
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Word learning in children: an examination of fast mapping.
    Heibeck TH; Markman EM
    Child Dev; 1987 Aug; 58(4):1021-34. PubMed ID: 3608655
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Learn locally, think globally. Exemplar variability supports higher-order generalization and word learning.
    Perry LK; Samuelson LK; Malloy LM; Schiffer RN
    Psychol Sci; 2010 Dec; 21(12):1894-902. PubMed ID: 21106892
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Statistical regularities in vocabulary guide language acquisition in connectionist models and 15-20-month-olds.
    Samuelson LK
    Dev Psychol; 2002 Nov; 38(6):1016-37. PubMed ID: 12428712
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Perceptual alignment contributes to referential transparency in indirect learning.
    Shao R; Gentner D
    Cognition; 2022 Jul; 224():105061. PubMed ID: 35183944
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.