These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
142 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31320725)
1. An asymmetry of treatment between lotteries involving gains and losses in rhesus monkeys. Nioche A; Bourgeois-Gironde S; Boraud T Sci Rep; 2019 Jul; 9(1):10441. PubMed ID: 31320725 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. The adaptive value of probability distortion and risk-seeking in macaques' decision-making. Nioche A; Rougier NP; Deffains M; Bourgeois-Gironde S; Ballesta S; Boraud T Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190668. PubMed ID: 33423627 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Probability Distortion Depends on Choice Sequence in Rhesus Monkeys. Ferrari-Toniolo S; Bujold PM; Schultz W J Neurosci; 2019 Apr; 39(15):2915-2929. PubMed ID: 30705103 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. On the Flexibility of Basic Risk Attitudes in Monkeys. Farashahi S; Azab H; Hayden B; Soltani A J Neurosci; 2018 May; 38(18):4383-4398. PubMed ID: 29626169 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Dissociable effects of basolateral amygdala lesions on decision making biases in rats when loss or gain is emphasized. Tremblay M; Cocker PJ; Hosking JG; Zeeb FD; Rogers RD; Winstanley CA Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2014 Dec; 14(4):1184-95. PubMed ID: 24668615 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Gambling in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): The effect of cues signaling risky choice outcomes. Smith TR; Beran MJ; Young ME Learn Behav; 2017 Sep; 45(3):288-299. PubMed ID: 28421468 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Tryptophan depletion alters the decision-making of healthy volunteers through altered processing of reward cues. Rogers RD; Tunbridge EM; Bhagwagar Z; Drevets WC; Sahakian BJ; Carter CS Neuropsychopharmacology; 2003 Jan; 28(1):153-62. PubMed ID: 12496952 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Monkeys (Sapajus apella and Macaca tonkeana) and great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes) play for the highest bid. Broihanne MH; Romain A; Call J; Thierry B; Wascher CAF; De Marco A; Verrier D; Dufour V J Comp Psychol; 2019 Aug; 133(3):301-312. PubMed ID: 30589294 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Betting on DBS: Effects of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation on risk taking and decision making in patients with Parkinson's disease. Brandt J; Rogerson M; Al-Joudi H; Reckess G; Shpritz B; Umeh CC; Aljehani N; Mills K; Mari Z Neuropsychology; 2015 Jul; 29(4):622-631. PubMed ID: 25486385 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Gambling for Gatorade: risk-sensitive decision making for fluid rewards in humans. Hayden BY; Platt ML Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):201-7. PubMed ID: 18719953 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Differential focus on probability and losses between young and older adults in risky decision-making. O'Brien EL; Hess TM Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn; 2020 Jul; 27(4):532-552. PubMed ID: 31355695 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. The role of memory in distinguishing risky decisions from experience and description. Madan CR; Ludvig EA; Spetch ML Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2017 Oct; 70(10):2048-2059. PubMed ID: 27602887 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. A developmental study of risky decisions on the cake gambling task: age and gender analyses of probability estimation and reward evaluation. Van Leijenhorst L; Westenberg PM; Crone EA Dev Neuropsychol; 2008; 33(2):179-96. PubMed ID: 18443976 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]