These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

106 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31372992)

  • 1. Preparation for certain and uncertain future outcomes in young children and three species of monkey.
    Suddendorf T; Watson K; Bogaart M; Redshaw J
    Dev Psychobiol; 2020 Mar; 62(2):191-201. PubMed ID: 31372992
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Preparatory responses to socially determined, mutually exclusive possibilities in chimpanzees and children.
    Suddendorf T; Crimston J; Redshaw J
    Biol Lett; 2017 Jun; 13(6):. PubMed ID: 28615352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Inference by exclusion in lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus), a hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas), capuchins (Sapajus apella), and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus).
    Marsh HL; Vining AQ; Levendoski EK; Judge PG
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Aug; 129(3):256-67. PubMed ID: 26010194
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus [sapajus] apella) show planning in a manual maze task.
    Prétôt L; Brosnan SF
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 Feb; 133(1):81-91. PubMed ID: 30234326
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Young children's capacity to imagine and prepare for certain and uncertain future outcomes.
    Redshaw J; Leamy T; Pincus P; Suddendorf T
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(9):e0202606. PubMed ID: 30180188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Semantic future thinking and executive functions at age 4: The moderating role of frontal brain electrical activity.
    Blankenship TL; Broomell APR; Ann Bell M
    Dev Psychobiol; 2018 Jul; 60(5):608-614. PubMed ID: 29785731
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Children's and Apes' Preparatory Responses to Two Mutually Exclusive Possibilities.
    Redshaw J; Suddendorf T
    Curr Biol; 2016 Jul; 26(13):1758-1762. PubMed ID: 27345164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Task switching in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) during computerized categorization tasks.
    Smith TR; Beran MJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2018 Jul; 44(3):229-246. PubMed ID: 29847983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) follow gaze around barriers: evidence for perspective taking?
    Amici F; Aureli F; Visalberghi E; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Nov; 123(4):368-74. PubMed ID: 19929105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Young Children From Three Diverse Cultures Spontaneously and Consistently Prepare for Alternative Future Possibilities.
    Redshaw J; Suddendorf T; Neldner K; Wilks M; Tomaselli K; Mushin I; Nielsen M
    Child Dev; 2019 Jan; 90(1):51-61. PubMed ID: 29737036
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Understanding deliberate practice in preschool-aged children.
    Davis JT; Cullen E; Suddendorf T
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2016; 69(2):361-80. PubMed ID: 26275104
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Convergence between G and g in three monkey species (Sapajus spp, Ateles geoffroyi, and Macaca fascicularis).
    Woodley Of Menie MA; Peñaherrera-Aguirre M
    J Comp Psychol; 2023 Feb; 137(1):62-73. PubMed ID: 35901380
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Not All Distractions Are the Same: Investigating Why Preschoolers Make Distraction Errors When Switching.
    Blakey E; Carroll DJ
    Child Dev; 2018 Mar; 89(2):609-619. PubMed ID: 28067417
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Young children can overcome their weak inhibitory control, if they conceptualize a task in the right way.
    Simpson A; Carroll DJ
    Cognition; 2018 Jan; 170():270-279. PubMed ID: 29096328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Episodic foresight beyond the very next event in 3- and 4-year-old children.
    Boden H; Labuschagne LG; Hinten AE; Scarf D
    Dev Psychobiol; 2017 Nov; 59(7):927-931. PubMed ID: 28731582
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The development of reasoning about the temporal and causal relations among past, present, and future events.
    Lohse K; Kalitschke T; Ruthmann K; Rakoczy H
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Oct; 138():54-70. PubMed ID: 26037402
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Young Children Detect and Avoid Logically Inconsistent Sources: The Importance of Communicative Context and Executive Function.
    Doebel S; Rowell SF; Koenig MA
    Child Dev; 2016 Nov; 87(6):1956-1970. PubMed ID: 27317511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Metacognitive Monitoring of Executive Control Engagement During Childhood.
    Chevalier N; Blaye A
    Child Dev; 2016 Jul; 87(4):1264-76. PubMed ID: 27084764
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Cognitive flexibility in young children: General or task-specific capacity?
    Deák GO; Wiseheart M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Oct; 138():31-53. PubMed ID: 26026421
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Children as Investigators of Brunerian "Possible Worlds". The Role of Narrative Scenarios in children's Argumentative Thinking.
    Iannaccone A; Perret-Clermont AN; Convertini J
    Integr Psychol Behav Sci; 2019 Dec; 53(4):679-693. PubMed ID: 31729627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.