These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

254 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31385241)

  • 21. The Nature of Task Set Representations in Working Memory.
    Kikumoto A; Mayr U
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2017 Nov; 29(11):1950-1961. PubMed ID: 28777061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Transfer after Dual n-Back Training Depends on Striatal Activation Change.
    Salminen T; Kühn S; Frensch PA; Schubert T
    J Neurosci; 2016 Sep; 36(39):10198-213. PubMed ID: 27683914
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Auditory working memory load impairs visual ventral stream processing: toward a unified model of attentional load.
    Klemen J; Büchel C; Bühler M; Menz MM; Rose M
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2010 Mar; 22(3):437-46. PubMed ID: 19302005
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Effects of working memory contents and perceptual load on distractor processing: When a response-related distractor is held in working memory.
    Koshino H
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2017 Jan; 172():19-25. PubMed ID: 27838400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Verbal makes it positive, spatial makes it negative: working memory biases judgments, attention, and moods.
    Storbeck J; Watson P
    Emotion; 2014 Dec; 14(6):1072-86. PubMed ID: 24999914
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Incidental threat during visuospatial working memory in adolescent anxiety: an emotional memory-guided saccade task.
    Mueller SC; Shechner T; Rosen D; Nelson EE; Pine DS; Ernst M
    Depress Anxiety; 2015 Apr; 32(4):289-95. PubMed ID: 25716665
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Stroop proactive control and task conflict are modulated by concurrent working memory load.
    Kalanthroff E; Avnit A; Henik A; Davelaar EJ; Usher M
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2015 Jun; 22(3):869-75. PubMed ID: 25257710
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Cognitive Flexibility Improves Memory for Delayed Intentions.
    Koslov SR; Mukerji A; Hedgpeth KR; Lewis-Peacock JA
    eNeuro; 2019; 6(6):. PubMed ID: 31601634
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Affective priming and cognitive load: Event-related potentials suggest an interplay of implicit affect misattribution and strategic inhibition.
    Gibbons H; Seib-Pfeifer LE; Koppehele-Gossel J; Schnuerch R
    Psychophysiology; 2018 Apr; 55(4):. PubMed ID: 28940207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. When flanker meets the n-back: What EEG and pupil dilation data reveal about the interplay between the two central-executive working memory functions inhibition and updating.
    Scharinger C; Soutschek A; Schubert T; Gerjets P
    Psychophysiology; 2015 Oct; 52(10):1293-304. PubMed ID: 26238380
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Phasic valence and arousal do not influence post-conflict adjustments in the Simon task.
    Dignath D; Janczyk M; Eder AB
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2017 Mar; 174():31-39. PubMed ID: 28135636
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex GABA Concentration in Humans Predicts Working Memory Load Processing Capacity.
    Yoon JH; Grandelis A; Maddock RJ
    J Neurosci; 2016 Nov; 36(46):11788-11794. PubMed ID: 27852785
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Neural evidence that inhibition is linked to the affective devaluation of distractors that match the contents of working memory.
    De Vito D; Al-Aidroos N; Fenske MJ
    Neuropsychologia; 2017 May; 99():259-269. PubMed ID: 28341131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Working memory capacity predicts conflict-task performance.
    Gulbinaite R; Johnson A
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2014; 67(7):1383-400. PubMed ID: 24199908
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Low cognitive load strengthens distractor interference while high load attenuates when cognitive load and distractor possess similar visual characteristics.
    Minamoto T; Shipstead Z; Osaka N; Engle RW
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2015 Jul; 77(5):1659-73. PubMed ID: 25813738
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Memory-driven attentional capture reveals the waxing and waning of working memory activation due to dual-task interference.
    Sasin E; Nieuwenstein M
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2016 Dec; 23(6):1891-1897. PubMed ID: 27125221
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The role of cognitive control mechanisms in selective attention towards emotional stimuli.
    Petrucci M; Pecchinenda A
    Cogn Emot; 2017 Nov; 31(7):1480-1492. PubMed ID: 27690176
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. High working memory load impairs selective attention: EEG signatures.
    Wei H; Zhou R
    Psychophysiology; 2020 Nov; 57(11):e13643. PubMed ID: 32725929
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Cognitive flexibility: A distinct element of performance impairment due to sleep deprivation.
    Honn KA; Hinson JM; Whitney P; Van Dongen HPA
    Accid Anal Prev; 2019 May; 126():191-197. PubMed ID: 29549968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. A secondary task is not always costly: Context-based guidance of visual search survives interference from a demanding working memory task.
    Annac E; Zang X; Müller HJ; Geyer T
    Br J Psychol; 2019 May; 110(2):381-399. PubMed ID: 30260470
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 13.