These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

106 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31470061)

  • 1. Evidence of non-circadian timing in a low response-cost daily Time-Place Learning task with pigeons Columba Livia.
    García-Gallardo D; Aguilar Guevara F; Moreno S; Hernández M; Carpio C
    Behav Processes; 2019 Nov; 168():103942. PubMed ID: 31470061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Effects of variable durations of food availability on interval time-place learning with pigeons Columba Livia.
    García-Gallardo D; Aguilar Guevara F; Canales C; Moreno S; Carpio C
    Behav Processes; 2020 Oct; 179():104192. PubMed ID: 32645386
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Effects of variable sequences of food availability on interval time-place learning by pigeons.
    García-Gallardo D; Carpio C
    Behav Processes; 2016 Sep; 130():53-64. PubMed ID: 27425658
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Time-space learning in homing pigeons (Columba livia): orientation to an artificial light source.
    Petruso EJ; Fuchs T; Bingman VP
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):181-8. PubMed ID: 17160343
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Rats use an ordinal timer in a daily time-place learning task.
    Carr JA; Wilkie DM
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1997 Apr; 23(2):232-47. PubMed ID: 9095544
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Rats are reluctant to use circadian timing in a daily time-place task.
    Carr JA; Wilkie DM
    Behav Processes; 1999 Mar; 44(3):287-99. PubMed ID: 24897230
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The effects of response cost and species-typical behaviors on a daily time-place learning task.
    Deibel SH; Thorpe CM
    Learn Behav; 2013 Mar; 41(1):42-53. PubMed ID: 22669816
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Consistent meal times improve performance on a daily time-place learning task.
    Wall K; Lewis LM; Deibel SH; Hallett D; Skinner DM; Thorpe CM
    Behav Processes; 2019 Mar; 160():26-32. PubMed ID: 30664921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Degree of representation of the matching concept in pigeons (Columba livia).
    Colombo M; Cottle A; Frost N
    J Comp Psychol; 2003 Sep; 117(3):246-56. PubMed ID: 14498800
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Representation of time in time-place learning.
    Pizzo MJ; Crystal JD
    Anim Learn Behav; 2002 Nov; 30(4):387-93. PubMed ID: 12593330
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Effects of reinforcer magnitude on responding under differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate schedules of rats and pigeons.
    Doughty AH; Richards JB
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2002 Jul; 78(1):17-30. PubMed ID: 12144310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The effects of lesions to the caudolateral neostriatum on sun compass based spatial learning in homing pigeons.
    Riters LV; Bingman VP
    Behav Brain Res; 1999 Jan; 98(1):1-15. PubMed ID: 10210517
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Information processing by pigeons (Columba livia): incentive as information.
    Shimp CP; Froehlich AL; Herbranson WT
    J Comp Psychol; 2007 Feb; 121(1):73-81. PubMed ID: 17324077
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Conditioned discrimination of magnetic inclination in a spatial-orientation arena task by homing pigeons (Columba livia).
    Mora CV; Acerbi ML; Bingman VP
    J Exp Biol; 2014 Dec; 217(Pt 23):4123-31. PubMed ID: 25278470
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Septal area lesions impair spatial working memory in homing pigeons (Columba livia).
    Peterson RM; Bingman VP
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2011 Sep; 96(2):353-60. PubMed ID: 21742046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Timing in pigeons: effects of the similarity between intertrial interval and gap in a timing signal.
    Kaiser DH; Zentall TR; Neiman E
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2002 Oct; 28(4):416-22. PubMed ID: 12395499
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Evidence for an alternation strategy in time-place learning.
    Pizzo MJ; Crystal JD
    Behav Processes; 2004 Nov; 67(3):533-7. PubMed ID: 15519002
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Pigeon (Columba livia) and rat (Rattus norvegicus) performance in the midsession reversal procedure depends upon cue dimensionality.
    McMillan N; Kirk CR; Roberts WA
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Nov; 128(4):357-66. PubMed ID: 25111628
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Strain differences in a high response-cost daily time-place learning task.
    Thorpe CM; Deibel SH; Reddigan JI; Fontaine CJ
    Behav Processes; 2012 Jul; 90(3):384-91. PubMed ID: 22542459
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Circadian time-place (or time-route) learning in rats with hippocampal lesions.
    Cole E; Mistlberger RE; Merza D; Trigiani LJ; Madularu D; Simundic A; Mumby DG
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2016 Dec; 136():236-243. PubMed ID: 27622983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.