These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

214 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31693776)

  • 21. Pragmatic responses to under-informative some-statements are not scalar implicatures.
    Kissine M; De Brabanter P
    Cognition; 2023 Aug; 237():105463. PubMed ID: 37060849
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. The logical syntax of number words: theory, acquisition and processing.
    Musolino J
    Cognition; 2009 Apr; 111(1):24-45. PubMed ID: 19217616
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Online pragmatic interpretations of scalar adjectives are affected by perceived speaker reliability.
    Gardner B; Dix S; Lawrence R; Morgan C; Sullivan A; Kurumada C
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(2):e0245130. PubMed ID: 33606683
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The Trouble With Quantifiers: Exploring Children's Deficits in Scalar Implicature.
    Horowitz AC; Schneider RM; Frank MC
    Child Dev; 2018 Nov; 89(6):e572-e593. PubMed ID: 29285759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Learning words from speakers with false beliefs.
    Papafragou A; Fairchild S; Cohen ML; Friedberg C
    J Child Lang; 2017 Jul; 44(4):905-923. PubMed ID: 27323650
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The Role of Executive Function and Theory of Mind in Pragmatic Computations.
    Fairchild S; Papafragou A
    Cogn Sci; 2021 Feb; 45(2):e12938. PubMed ID: 33616218
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Perspective-taking in deriving implicatures: The listener's perspective is important too.
    Katsos N; de Linares BG; Ostashchenko E; Wilson E
    Cognition; 2023 Dec; 241():105582. PubMed ID: 37678083
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Challenges with computing scalar and ad-hoc implicatures in Mandarin-speaking 4-8-year-old autistic children.
    Su YE; Jiang Y
    J Commun Disord; 2024; 110():106427. PubMed ID: 38729067
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Inferring word meanings by assuming that speakers are informative.
    Frank MC; Goodman ND
    Cogn Psychol; 2014 Dec; 75():80-96. PubMed ID: 25238461
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Pragmatic tolerance: implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature.
    Katsos N; Bishop DV
    Cognition; 2011 Jul; 120(1):67-81. PubMed ID: 21429481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Are scalar implicatures automatically processed and different for each individual? A mismatch negativity (MMN) study.
    Zhao M; Liu T; Chen G; Chen F
    Brain Res; 2015 Mar; 1599():137-49. PubMed ID: 25542387
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Speaker knowledge influences the comprehension of pragmatic inferences.
    Bergen L; Grodner DJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2012 Sep; 38(5):1450-60. PubMed ID: 22545611
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Taking the epistemic step: toward a model of on-line access to conversational implicatures.
    Breheny R; Ferguson HJ; Katsos N
    Cognition; 2013 Mar; 126(3):423-40. PubMed ID: 23291422
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Some Pieces Are Missing: Implicature Production in Children.
    Eiteljoerge SFV; Pouscoulous N; Lieven EVM
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():1928. PubMed ID: 30405468
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Neural correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in autism spectrum disorders.
    Tesink CM; Buitelaar JK; Petersson KM; van der Gaag RJ; Kan CC; Tendolkar I; Hagoort P
    Brain; 2009 Jul; 132(Pt 7):1941-52. PubMed ID: 19423680
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. The object of my desire: Five-year-olds rapidly reason about a speaker's desire during referential communication.
    San Juan V; Chambers CG; Berman J; Humphry C; Graham SA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Oct; 162():101-119. PubMed ID: 28600922
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Neural correlates of fine-grained meaning distinctions: An fMRI investigation of scalar quantifiers.
    Zhan J; Jiang X; Politzer-Ahles S; Zhou X
    Hum Brain Mapp; 2017 Aug; 38(8):3848-3864. PubMed ID: 28481027
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Language complexity modulates 8- and 10-year-olds' success at using their theory of mind abilities in a communication task.
    Wang JJ; Ali M; Frisson S; Apperly IA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Sep; 149():62-71. PubMed ID: 26477598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. The communicative style of a speaker can affect language comprehension? ERP evidence from the comprehension of irony.
    Regel S; Coulson S; Gunter TC
    Brain Res; 2010 Jan; 1311():121-35. PubMed ID: 19900421
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. When some is not every: dissociating scalar implicature generation and mismatch.
    Shetreet E; Chierchia G; Gaab N
    Hum Brain Mapp; 2014 Apr; 35(4):1503-14. PubMed ID: 23568365
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.