These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

195 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 31722259)

  • 1. Dyad training protocols and the development of a motor sequence representation.
    Panzer S; Haab T; Massing M; Pfeifer C; Shea CH
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2019 Oct; 201():102947. PubMed ID: 31722259
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The influence of eye-movements on the development of a movement sequence representation during observational and physical practice.
    Massing M; Blandin Y; Panzer S
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2018 Jan; 182():1-8. PubMed ID: 29107928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Across-task binding: The development of a representation in learning a continuous movement sequence.
    Panzer S; Pfeifer C; Daniel L; Gaschler R; Haider H; Shea CH
    Hum Mov Sci; 2024 Apr; 94():103195. PubMed ID: 38359609
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Scheduling observational and physical practice: influence on the coding of simple motor sequences.
    Ellenbuerger T; Boutin A; Blandin Y; Shea CH; Panzer S
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2012; 65(7):1260-73. PubMed ID: 22494362
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Observing different model types interspersed with physical practice has no effect on consolidation or motor learning of an elbow flexion-extension task.
    Moore CM; Lelievre N; Ste-Marie DM
    Hum Mov Sci; 2019 Feb; 63():96-107. PubMed ID: 30508690
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Observational training in visual half-fields and the coding of movement sequences.
    Ellenbuerger T; Boutin A; Panzer S; Blandin Y; Fischer L; Schorer J; Shea CH
    Hum Mov Sci; 2012 Dec; 31(6):1436-48. PubMed ID: 22939848
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Dual-Task and Single-Task Practice Does Not Influence the Attentional Demands of Movement Sequence Representations.
    Pfeifer C; Harenz J; Shea CH; Panzer S
    J Mot Behav; 2024; 56(4):462-474. PubMed ID: 38484757
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The impact of concurrent visual feedback on coding of on-line and pre-planned movement sequences.
    Leinen P; Shea CH; Panzer S
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2015 Feb; 155():92-100. PubMed ID: 25594377
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Inter-manual transfer and practice: coding of simple motor sequences.
    Panzer S; Krueger M; Muehlbauer T; Kovacs AJ; Shea CH
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2009 Jun; 131(2):99-109. PubMed ID: 19389659
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Practice makes transfer of motor skills imperfect.
    Boutin A; Badets A; Salesse RN; Fries U; Panzer S; Blandin Y
    Psychol Res; 2012 Sep; 76(5):611-25. PubMed ID: 21671102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Attentional Demand of a Movement Sequence Guided by Visual-Spatial and by Motor Representations.
    Pfeifer C; Panzer S; Shea CH
    J Mot Behav; 2023; 55(1):58-67. PubMed ID: 35878952
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Observation and physical practice: coding of simple motor sequences.
    Gruetzmacher N; Panzer S; Blandin Y; Shea CH
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2011 Jun; 64(6):1111-23. PubMed ID: 21302186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intramanual transfer.
    Hayes SJ; Elliott D; Andrew M; Roberts JW; Bennett SJ
    Exp Brain Res; 2012 Sep; 221(4):459-66. PubMed ID: 22821082
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Observation learning versus physical practice leads to different consolidation outcomes in a movement timing task.
    Trempe M; Sabourin M; Rohbanfard H; Proteau L
    Exp Brain Res; 2011 Mar; 209(2):181-92. PubMed ID: 21279634
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. General motor representations are developed during action-observation.
    Hayes SJ; Elliott D; Bennett SJ
    Exp Brain Res; 2010 Jul; 204(2):199-206. PubMed ID: 20502885
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning.
    Bapi RS; Doya K; Harner AM
    Exp Brain Res; 2000 May; 132(2):149-62. PubMed ID: 10853941
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Enhancing motor learning through dyad practice: contributions of observation and dialogue.
    Granados C; Wulf G
    Res Q Exerc Sport; 2007 Jun; 78(3):197-203. PubMed ID: 17679493
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Learning and interlimb transfer of new gait patterns are facilitated by distributed practice across days.
    Krishnan C
    Gait Posture; 2019 May; 70():84-89. PubMed ID: 30831544
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Enhancing Training Efficiency and Effectiveness Through the Use of Dyad Training.
    Shea CH; Wulf G; Whitacre C
    J Mot Behav; 1999 Jun; 31(2):119-125. PubMed ID: 11177626
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The use of motor imagery training to retain the performance improvement following physical practice in the elderly.
    Ruffino C; Bourrelier J; Papaxanthis C; Mourey F; Lebon F
    Exp Brain Res; 2019 Jun; 237(6):1375-1382. PubMed ID: 30877341
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.