These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

136 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 32042276)

  • 1. Non-Linguistic Grammar Learning by 12-Month-Old Infants: Evidence for Constraints on Learning.
    Santolin C; Saffran JR
    J Cogn Dev; 2019; 20(3):433-441. PubMed ID: 32042276
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Precursors to natural grammar learning: preliminary evidence from 4-month-old infants.
    Friederici AD; Mueller JL; Oberecker R
    PLoS One; 2011 Mar; 6(3):e17920. PubMed ID: 21445341
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Finding phrases: On the role of co-verbal facial information in learning word order in infancy.
    de la Cruz-Pavía I; Gervain J; Vatikiotis-Bateson E; Werker JF
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(11):e0224786. PubMed ID: 31710615
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Behavioral and Imaging Studies of Infant Artificial Grammar Learning.
    Gervain J; de la Cruz-Pavía I; Gerken L
    Top Cogn Sci; 2020 Jul; 12(3):815-827. PubMed ID: 30554481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Learning and generalizing non-adjacent dependencies in 18-month-olds: A mechanism for language acquisition?
    Grama I; Wijnen F
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(10):e0204481. PubMed ID: 30307956
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge.
    Gomez RL; Gerken L
    Cognition; 1999 Mar; 70(2):109-35. PubMed ID: 10349760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Artificial grammar learning in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) in varying stimulus contexts.
    Neiworth JJ; London JM; Flynn MJ; Rupert DD; Alldritt O; Hyde C
    J Comp Psychol; 2017 May; 131(2):128-138. PubMed ID: 28277720
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. What do animals learn in artificial grammar studies?
    Beckers GJL; Berwick RC; Okanoya K; Bolhuis JJ
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2017 Oct; 81(Pt B):238-246. PubMed ID: 28017840
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Artificial Grammar Learning Capabilities in an Abstract Visual Task Match Requirements for Linguistic Syntax.
    Westphal-Fitch G; Giustolisi B; Cecchetto C; Martin JS; Fitch WT
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():1210. PubMed ID: 30087630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Grammatical pattern learning by human infants and cotton-top tamarin monkeys.
    Saffran J; Hauser M; Seibel R; Kapfhamer J; Tsao F; Cushman F
    Cognition; 2008 May; 107(2):479-500. PubMed ID: 18082676
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Budgerigars and zebra finches differ in how they generalize in an artificial grammar learning experiment.
    Spierings MJ; Ten Cate C
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2016 Jul; 113(27):E3977-84. PubMed ID: 27325756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The Developmental Trajectory of Nonadjacent Dependency Learning.
    Gómez R; Maye J
    Infancy; 2005 Mar; 7(2):183-206. PubMed ID: 33430549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Tuning in to non-adjacencies: Exposure to learnable patterns supports discovering otherwise difficult structures.
    Zettersten M; Potter CE; Saffran JR
    Cognition; 2020 Sep; 202():104283. PubMed ID: 32623134
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Twelve-month-old infants benefit from prior experience in statistical learning.
    Lany J; Gómez RL
    Psychol Sci; 2008 Dec; 19(12):1247-52. PubMed ID: 19121132
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Words in a sea of sounds: the output of infant statistical learning.
    Saffran JR
    Cognition; 2001 Sep; 81(2):149-69. PubMed ID: 11376640
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Learning language with the wrong neural scaffolding: the cost of neural commitment to sounds.
    Finn AS; Hudson Kam CL; Ettlinger M; Vytlacil J; D'Esposito M
    Front Syst Neurosci; 2013; 7():85. PubMed ID: 24273497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Syntactic transfer in artificial grammar learning.
    Beesley T; Wills AJ; Le Pelley ME
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2010 Feb; 17(1):122-8. PubMed ID: 20081172
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Non-adjacent Dependency Learning in Humans and Other Animals.
    Wilson B; Spierings M; Ravignani A; Mueller JL; Mintz TH; Wijnen F; van der Kant A; Smith K; Rey A
    Top Cogn Sci; 2020 Jul; 12(3):843-858. PubMed ID: 32729673
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Memory for nonadjacent dependencies in the first year of life and its relation to sleep.
    Friedrich M; Mölle M; Born J; Friederici AD
    Nat Commun; 2022 Dec; 13(1):7896. PubMed ID: 36550131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.