These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
23. Perception of surface curvature and direction of illumination from patterns of shading. Todd JT; Mingolla E J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 1983 Aug; 9(4):583-95. PubMed ID: 6224894 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
24. The effects of smooth occlusions and directions of illumination on the visual perception of 3-D shape from shading. Egan EJ; Todd JT J Vis; 2015 Feb; 15(2):. PubMed ID: 25761340 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
25. Relative flattening between velvet and matte 3D shapes: evidence for similar shape-from-shading computations. Wijntjes MW; Doerschner K; Kucukoglu G; Pont SC J Vis; 2012 Jan; 12(1):. PubMed ID: 22214564 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
26. Light source position in the perception of object shape. Berbaum K; Bever T; Chung CS Perception; 1983; 12(4):411-6. PubMed ID: 6672736 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
27. Light direction from shad(ow)ed random Gaussian surfaces. Koenderink JJ; van Doorn AJ; Pont SC Perception; 2004; 33(12):1405-20. PubMed ID: 15729909 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
28. The darker-is-deeper heuristic for the perception of 3D shape from shading: Is it perceptually or ecologically valid? Todd JT; Egan EJ; Kallie CS J Vis; 2015; 15(15):2. PubMed ID: 26562310 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
31. The efficacy of local luminance amplitude in disambiguating the origin of luminance signals depends on carrier frequency: further evidence for the active role of second-order vision in layer decomposition. Sun P; Schofield AJ Vision Res; 2011 Mar; 51(5):496-507. PubMed ID: 21272596 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
32. Illusory 3-D rotation induced by dynamic image shading. Caudek C; Domini F; Di Luca M Percept Psychophys; 2002 Apr; 64(3):366-79. PubMed ID: 12049278 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
33. Envelope-responsive neurons in areas 17 and 18 of cat. Zhou YX; Baker CL J Neurophysiol; 1994 Nov; 72(5):2134-50. PubMed ID: 7884449 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
35. A cue-free method to probe human lighting biases. Mazzilli G; Schofield AJ Perception; 2013; 42(9):932-40. PubMed ID: 24386713 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
36. Shape from shading from images rendered with various surface types and light fields. Khang BG; Koenderink JJ; Kappers AM Perception; 2007; 36(8):1191-213. PubMed ID: 17972483 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
37. Gradient representation and perception in the early visual system--a novel account of Mach band formation. Keil MS; Cristóbal G; Neumann H Vision Res; 2006 Sep; 46(17):2659-74. PubMed ID: 16603218 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
38. Interactions between luminance and color signals: effects on shape. Clery S; Bloj M; Harris JM J Vis; 2013 Apr; 13(5):. PubMed ID: 23599420 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
39. Simultaneous recognition of two spatial-frequency components. Hirsch J; Hylton R; Graham N Vision Res; 1982; 22(3):365-75. PubMed ID: 7090190 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
40. Spatial frequency range in the detection process--1. Narrow bars. Brogan D Ophthalmic Physiol Opt; 1985; 5(2):125-35. PubMed ID: 4022644 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Previous] [Next] [New Search]