These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

199 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 32238036)

  • 1. Adolescents are delayed at inferring complex social intentions in others, but not basic (false) beliefs: An eye-movement investigation.
    Symeonidou I; Dumontheil I; Ferguson HJ; Breheny R
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2020 Oct; 73(10):1640-1659. PubMed ID: 32238036
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Perceptual Access Reasoning (PAR) in Developing a Representational Theory of Mind.
    Fabricius WV; Gonzales CR; Pesch A; Weimer AA; Pugliese J; Carroll K; Bolnick RR; Kupfer AS; Eisenberg N; Spinrad TL
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 86(3):7-154. PubMed ID: 34580875
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Theory of mind development from adolescence to adulthood: Testing the two-component model.
    Meinhardt-Injac B; Daum MM; Meinhardt G
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2020 Jun; 38(2):289-303. PubMed ID: 31960462
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Theory of Mind in aging: Comparing cognitive and affective components in the faux pas test.
    Bottiroli S; Cavallini E; Ceccato I; Vecchi T; Lecce S
    Arch Gerontol Geriatr; 2016; 62():152-62. PubMed ID: 26434925
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Beyond false belief: theory of mind in young, young-old, and old-old adults.
    Cavallini E; Lecce S; Bottiroli S; Palladino P; Pagnin A
    Int J Aging Hum Dev; 2013; 76(3):181-98. PubMed ID: 23781702
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Perspective-taking and social inferences in adolescents, young adults, and older adults.
    De Lillo M; Ferguson HJ
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2023 May; 152(5):1420-1438. PubMed ID: 36862493
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning.
    Durrleman S; Franck J
    J Commun Disord; 2015; 54():15-31. PubMed ID: 25637130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Theory-of-mind in adolescents and young adults with Alström syndrome.
    Frölander HE; Möller C; Marshall JD; Sundqvist A; Rönnåsen B; Falkensson L; Lyxell B
    Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol; 2014 Mar; 78(3):530-6. PubMed ID: 24485176
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Causal evidence of right temporal parietal junction involvement in implicit Theory of Mind processing.
    Filmer HL; Fox A; Dux PE
    Neuroimage; 2019 Aug; 196():329-336. PubMed ID: 30981855
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Implicit false belief tracking is preserved in late adulthood.
    Grainger SA; Henry JD; Naughtin CK; Comino MS; Dux PE
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2018 Sep; 71(9):1980-1987. PubMed ID: 30117384
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Implicit false-belief processing in the human brain.
    Schneider D; Slaughter VP; Becker SI; Dux PE
    Neuroimage; 2014 Nov; 101():268-75. PubMed ID: 25042446
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Deconstructing Theory-of-Mind Impairment in High-Functioning Adults with Autism.
    Rosenthal IA; Hutcherson CA; Adolphs R; Stanley DA
    Curr Biol; 2019 Feb; 29(3):513-519.e6. PubMed ID: 30686740
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Does interference between self and other perspectives in theory of mind tasks reflect a common underlying process? Evidence from individual differences in theory of mind and inhibitory control.
    Qureshi AW; Monk RL; Samson D; Apperly IA
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2020 Feb; 27(1):178-190. PubMed ID: 31429057
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Impaired theory of mind in Chinese children and adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy: Association with behavioral manifestations of executive dysfunction.
    Zhang T; Chen L; Wang Y; Zhang M; Wang L; Xu X; Xiao G; Chen J; Shen Y; Zhou N
    Epilepsy Behav; 2018 Feb; 79():205-212. PubMed ID: 29309954
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Social representations and contextual adjustments as two distinct components of the Theory of Mind brain network: Evidence from the REMICS task.
    Lavoie MA; Vistoli D; Sutliff S; Jackson PL; Achim AM
    Cortex; 2016 Aug; 81():176-91. PubMed ID: 27236373
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders.
    Schneider D; Slaughter VP; Bayliss AP; Dux PE
    Cognition; 2013 Nov; 129(2):410-7. PubMed ID: 23994318
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Evidence for deficits on different components of theory of mind in Huntington's disease.
    Allain P; Havet-Thomassin V; Verny C; Gohier B; Lancelot C; Besnard J; Fasotti L; Le Gall D
    Neuropsychology; 2011 Nov; 25(6):741-51. PubMed ID: 21728429
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Executive function predicts theory of mind but not social verbal communication in school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder.
    Kouklari EC; Tsermentseli S; Auyeung B
    Res Dev Disabil; 2018 May; 76():12-24. PubMed ID: 29547763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The relationship between theory of mind and the executive functions: Evidence from patients with frontal lobe damage.
    Yeh ZT; Tsai MC; Tsai MD; Lo CY; Wang KC
    Appl Neuropsychol Adult; 2017; 24(4):342-349. PubMed ID: 27282192
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Exploring theory of mind after severe traumatic brain injury.
    Muller F; Simion A; Reviriego E; Galera C; Mazaux JM; Barat M; Joseph PA
    Cortex; 2010 Oct; 46(9):1088-99. PubMed ID: 19828142
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.