These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

116 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 32380744)

  • 1. Attentional Flexibility Predicts A-Not-B Task Performance in 14-Month-Old-Infants: A Head-Mounted Eye Tracking Study.
    Mulder H; Van Houdt CA; J M Van der Ham I; Van der Stigchel S; Oudgenoeg-Paz O
    Brain Sci; 2020 May; 10(5):. PubMed ID: 32380744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Individual differences in 10-month-olds' performance on the A-not-B task.
    Johansson M; Forssman L; Bohlin G
    Scand J Psychol; 2014 Apr; 55(2):130-5. PubMed ID: 24646045
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Attentional Predictors of 5-month-olds' Performance on a Looking A-not-B Task.
    Marcovitch S; Clearfield MW; Swingler M; Calkins SD; Bell MA
    Infant Child Dev; 2016; 25(4):233-246. PubMed ID: 27642263
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. What Do Young Infants Do During Eye-Tracking Experiments? IP-BET - A Coding Scheme for Quantifying Spontaneous Infant and Parent Behaviour.
    Tomalski P; Malinowska-Korczak A
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():764. PubMed ID: 32411051
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Attention demands influence 10- and 12-month-old infants' perseverative behavior.
    Watanabe H; Forssman L; Green D; Bohlin G; von Hofsten C
    Dev Psychol; 2012 Jan; 48(1):46-55. PubMed ID: 21910526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Is selective attention the basis for selective imitation in infants? An eye-tracking study of deferred imitation with 12-month-olds.
    Kolling T; Oturai G; Knopf M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Aug; 124():18-35. PubMed ID: 24727296
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Face detection in 2- to 6-month-old infants is influenced by gaze direction and species.
    Simpson EA; Maylott SE; Mitsven SG; Zeng G; Jakobsen KV
    Dev Sci; 2020 Mar; 23(2):e12902. PubMed ID: 31505079
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Focused attention predicts visual working memory performance in 13-month-old infants: A pupillometric study.
    Cheng C; Kaldy Z; Blaser E
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2019 Apr; 36():100616. PubMed ID: 30769261
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Infants' and adults' looking behavior does not indicate perceptual distraction for constrained modelled actions - An eye-tracking study.
    Buttelmann D; Schieler A; Wetzel N; Widmann A
    Infant Behav Dev; 2017 May; 47():103-111. PubMed ID: 28391125
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Head and eye movements affect object processing in 4-month-old infants more than an artificial orientation cue.
    Wahl S; Michel C; Pauen S; Hoehl S
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2013 Jun; 31(Pt 2):212-30. PubMed ID: 23659892
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Observing Third-Party Attentional Relationships Affects Infants' Gaze Following: An Eye-Tracking Study.
    Meng X; Uto Y; Hashiya K
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():2065. PubMed ID: 28149284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Attentional inertia and delayed orienting of spatial attention in task-switching.
    Longman CS; Lavric A; Munteanu C; Monsell S
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2014 Aug; 40(4):1580-602. PubMed ID: 24842065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Reactive and anticipatory looking in 6-month-old infants during a visual expectation paradigm.
    Quan J; Bureau JF; Abdul Malik AB; Wong J; Rifkin-Graboi A
    Data Brief; 2017 Oct; 14():713-719. PubMed ID: 28932776
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Beyond screen time: Using head-mounted eye tracking to study natural behavior.
    Franchak JM; Yu C
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2022; 62():61-91. PubMed ID: 35249686
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Self-paced preparation for a task switch eliminates attentional inertia but not the performance switch cost.
    Longman CS; Lavric A; Monsell S
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2017 Jun; 43(6):862-873. PubMed ID: 27936844
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy.
    Sheese BE; Rothbart MK; Posner MI; White LK; Fraundorf SH
    Infant Behav Dev; 2008 Sep; 31(3):501-10. PubMed ID: 18406466
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Cognitive flexibility: A distinct element of performance impairment due to sleep deprivation.
    Honn KA; Hinson JM; Whitney P; Van Dongen HPA
    Accid Anal Prev; 2019 May; 126():191-197. PubMed ID: 29549968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Pupil size signals mental effort deployed during multiple object tracking and predicts brain activity in the dorsal attention network and the locus coeruleus.
    Alnæs D; Sneve MH; Espeseth T; Endestad T; van de Pavert SH; Laeng B
    J Vis; 2014 Apr; 14(4):. PubMed ID: 24692319
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Development of an eye-tracking method to assess mental set switching in people with aphasia.
    Heuer S; Pinke ML
    Brain Inj; 2017; 31(5):686-696. PubMed ID: 28406332
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.