These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
112 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 32654276)
1. The value of proactive goal setting and choice in 3- to 7-year-olds' use of working memory gating strategies in a naturalistic task. Freier L; Gupta P; Badre D; Amso D Dev Sci; 2021 Jan; 24(1):e13017. PubMed ID: 32654276 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Changes in working memory influence the transition from reactive to proactive cognitive control during childhood. Troller-Renfree SV; Buzzell GA; Fox NA Dev Sci; 2020 Nov; 23(6):e12959. PubMed ID: 32141641 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Five-Year-Old Children's Working Memory Can Be Improved When Children Act On A Transparent Goal Cue. Fitamen C; Blaye A; Camos V Sci Rep; 2019 Oct; 9(1):15342. PubMed ID: 31653944 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. The impact of cognitive control on children's goal monitoring in a time-based prospective memory task. Mahy CE; Voigt B; Ballhausen N; Schnitzspahn K; Ellis J; Kliegel M Child Neuropsychol; 2015; 21(6):823-39. PubMed ID: 25342074 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Evidencing the developmental shift from reactive to proactive control in early childhood and its relationship to working memory. Gonthier C; Zira M; Colé P; Blaye A J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Jan; 177():1-16. PubMed ID: 30165288 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Six- to eight-year-olds' performance in the Heart and Flower task: Emerging proactive cognitive control. Roebers CM Front Psychol; 2022; 13():923615. PubMed ID: 36033019 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Reactive and proactive control processes in voluntary task choice. Mittelstädt V; Mackenzie IG; Braun DA; Arrington CM Mem Cognit; 2024 Feb; 52(2):417-429. PubMed ID: 37798607 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. The development of strategy use in elementary school children: working memory and individual differences. Imbo I; Vandierendonck A J Exp Child Psychol; 2007 Apr; 96(4):284-309. PubMed ID: 17046017 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Explore versus store: Children strategically trade off reliance on exploration versus working memory during a complex task. Kenderla P; Kibbe MM J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Jan; 225():105535. PubMed ID: 36041236 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Proactive control in early and middle childhood: An ERP study. Elke S; Wiebe SA Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2017 Aug; 26():28-38. PubMed ID: 28436833 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Consistent use of proactive control and relation with academic achievement in childhood. Kubota M; Hadley LV; Schaeffner S; Könen T; Meaney JA; Auyeung B; Morey CC; Karbach J; Chevalier N Cognition; 2020 Oct; 203():104329. PubMed ID: 32526518 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Central executive involvement in children's spatial memory. Ang SY; Lee K Memory; 2008 Nov; 16(8):918-33. PubMed ID: 18802804 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. The development of future thinking: young children's ability to construct event sequences to achieve future goals. Prabhakar J; Hudson JA J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Nov; 127():95-109. PubMed ID: 24786765 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Age-related changes in the temporal dynamics of executive control: a study in 5- and 6-year-old children. Lucenet J; Blaye A Front Psychol; 2014; 5():831. PubMed ID: 25120523 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Task context load induces reactive cognitive control: An fMRI study on cortical and brain stem activity. Mäki-Marttunen V; Hagen T; Espeseth T Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2019 Aug; 19(4):945-965. PubMed ID: 30659515 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Cognitive control in context: working memory capacity and proactive control. Redick TS Acta Psychol (Amst); 2014 Jan; 145():1-9. PubMed ID: 24240136 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]