These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

180 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33022842)

  • 1. What are the building blocks of parent-infant coordinated attention in free-flowing interaction?
    Abney DH; Suanda SH; Smith LB; Yu C
    Infancy; 2020 Nov; 25(6):871-887. PubMed ID: 33022842
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Infant sustained attention but not joint attention to objects at 9 months predicts vocabulary at 12 and 15 months.
    Yu C; Suanda SH; Smith LB
    Dev Sci; 2019 Jan; 22(1):e12735. PubMed ID: 30255968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Driven from distraction: how infants respond to parents' attempts to elicit and re-direct their attention.
    Deák GO; Walden TA; Kaiser MY; Lewis A
    Infant Behav Dev; 2008 Jan; 31(1):34-50. PubMed ID: 17692386
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Multimodal parent behaviors within joint attention support sustained attention in infants.
    Suarez-Rivera C; Smith LB; Yu C
    Dev Psychol; 2019 Jan; 55(1):96-109. PubMed ID: 30489136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Joint attention without gaze following: human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination.
    Yu C; Smith LB
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(11):e79659. PubMed ID: 24236151
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. What leads to coordinated attention in parent-toddler interactions? Children's hearing status matters.
    Chen CH; Castellanos I; Yu C; Houston DM
    Dev Sci; 2020 May; 23(3):e12919. PubMed ID: 31680414
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Attention to the mouth and gaze following in infancy predict language development.
    Tenenbaum EJ; Sobel DM; Sheinkopf SJ; Shah RJ; Malle BF; Morgan JL
    J Child Lang; 2015 Nov; 42(6):1173-90. PubMed ID: 25403090
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Hand-Eye Coordination Predicts Joint Attention.
    Yu C; Smith LB
    Child Dev; 2017 Nov; 88(6):2060-2078. PubMed ID: 28186339
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Effects of children's hearing loss on the synchrony between parents' object naming and children's attention.
    Chen CH; Castellanos I; Yu C; Houston DM
    Infant Behav Dev; 2019 Nov; 57():101322. PubMed ID: 31102946
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Looking is not enough: Multimodal attention supports the real-time learning of new words.
    Schroer SE; Yu C
    Dev Sci; 2023 Mar; 26(2):e13290. PubMed ID: 35617054
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Why the parent's gaze is so powerful in organizing the infant's gaze: The relationship between parental referential cues and infant object looking.
    Sun L; Yoshida H
    Infancy; 2022 Jul; 27(4):780-808. PubMed ID: 35575583
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Linking Joint Attention with Hand-Eye Coordination - A Sensorimotor Approach to Understanding Child-Parent Social Interaction.
    Yu C; Smith LB
    Cogsci; 2015 Jul; 2015():2763-2768. PubMed ID: 29226280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Contingencies Between Infants' Gaze, Vocal, and Manual Actions and Mothers' Object-Naming: Longitudinal Changes From 4 to 9 Months.
    Chang L; de Barbaro K; Deák G
    Dev Neuropsychol; 2016; 41(5-8):342-361. PubMed ID: 28253035
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Touchscreens can promote infant object-interlocutor reference switching.
    Hudspeth KM; Lewis C
    Infant Behav Dev; 2024 Mar; 74():101914. PubMed ID: 38065036
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Associations between technoference, quality of parent-infant interactions, and infants' vocabulary development.
    Corkin MT; Henderson AME; Peterson ER; Kennedy-Costantini S; Sharplin HS; Morrison S
    Infant Behav Dev; 2021 Aug; 64():101611. PubMed ID: 34303915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The role of maternal affect attunement in dyadic and triadic communication.
    Legerstee M; Markova G; Fisher T
    Infant Behav Dev; 2007 May; 30(2):296-306. PubMed ID: 17400046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Infants' intentionally communicative vocalizations elicit responses from caregivers and are the best predictors of the transition to language: A longitudinal investigation of infants' vocalizations, gestures and word production.
    Donnellan E; Bannard C; McGillion ML; Slocombe KE; Matthews D
    Dev Sci; 2020 Jan; 23(1):e12843. PubMed ID: 31045301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Individual differences in object-processing explain the relationship between early gaze-following and later language development.
    Okumura Y; Kanakogi Y; Kobayashi T; Itakura S
    Cognition; 2017 Sep; 166():418-424. PubMed ID: 28624708
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Building language learning: Relations between infant attention and social contingency in the first year of life.
    Masek LR; Edgar EV; McMillan BTM; Todd JT; Golinkoff RM; Bahrick LE; Hirsh-Pasek K
    Infant Behav Dev; 2024 Jun; 75():101933. PubMed ID: 38507845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. See and be seen: Infant-caregiver social looking during locomotor free play.
    Franchak JM; Kretch KS; Adolph KE
    Dev Sci; 2018 Jul; 21(4):e12626. PubMed ID: 29071760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.