These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

175 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33104751)

  • 1. Why are listeners sometimes (but not always) egocentric? Making inferences about using others' perspective in referential communication.
    Wang JJ; Ciranova N; Woods B; Apperly IA
    PLoS One; 2020; 15(10):e0240521. PubMed ID: 33104751
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Task constraints distinguish perspective inferences from perspective use during discourse interpretation in a false belief task.
    Ferguson HJ; Apperly I; Ahmad J; Bindemann M; Cane J
    Cognition; 2015 Jun; 139():50-70. PubMed ID: 25800351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The cognitive demands of remembering a speaker's perspective and managing common ground size modulate 8- and 10-year-olds' perspective-taking abilities.
    Zhao L; Wang JJ; Apperly IA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Oct; 174():130-149. PubMed ID: 29940398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Dissociation of understanding from applying others' false beliefs in remitted schizophrenia: evidence from a computerized referential communication task.
    Wang YG; Roberts DL; Xu BH
    BMC Psychiatry; 2013 May; 13():141. PubMed ID: 23683146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Communicative functions of directional verbal probabilities: Speaker's choice, listener's inference, and reference points.
    Honda H; Yamagishi K
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2017 Oct; 70(10):2141-2158. PubMed ID: 27550640
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Perspective-taking across cultures: shared biases in Taiwanese and British adults.
    Wang JJ; Tseng P; Juan CH; Frisson S; Apperly IA
    R Soc Open Sci; 2019 Nov; 6(11):190540. PubMed ID: 31827820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Adjustment of speaker's referential expressions to an addressee's likely knowledge and link with theory of mind abilities.
    Achim AM; Fossard M; Couture S; Achim A
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():823. PubMed ID: 26136711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Correction: Why are listeners sometimes (but not always) egocentric? Making inferences about using others' perspective in referential communication.
    Jessica Wang J; Ciranova N; Woods B; Apperly IA
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(11):e0259617. PubMed ID: 34723995
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Looking to understand: the coupling between speakers' and listeners' eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension.
    Richardson DC; Dale R
    Cogn Sci; 2005 Nov; 29(6):1045-60. PubMed ID: 21702802
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Privileged versus shared knowledge about object identity in real-time referential processing.
    Mozuraitis M; Chambers CG; Daneman M
    Cognition; 2015 Sep; 142():148-65. PubMed ID: 26048297
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults' ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker.
    Apperly IA; Carroll DJ; Samson D; Humphreys GW; Qureshi A; Moffitt G
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2010 Jun; 63(6):1201-17. PubMed ID: 19838912
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Language complexity modulates 8- and 10-year-olds' success at using their theory of mind abilities in a communication task.
    Wang JJ; Ali M; Frisson S; Apperly IA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Sep; 149():62-71. PubMed ID: 26477598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: the case of irony.
    Spotorno N; Koun E; Prado J; Van Der Henst JB; Noveck IA
    Neuroimage; 2012 Oct; 63(1):25-39. PubMed ID: 22766167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Expectations about the source of a speaker's accent affect accent adaptation.
    Vaughn CR
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2019 May; 145(5):3218. PubMed ID: 31153344
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Two independent sources of difficulty in perspective-taking/theory of mind tasks.
    Samuel S; Cole GG; Eacott MJ
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2020 Dec; 27(6):1341-1347. PubMed ID: 32779118
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Neural bases of social communicative intentions in speech.
    Hellbernd N; Sammler D
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2018 Jun; 13(6):604-615. PubMed ID: 29771359
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Words are not enough: how preschoolers' integration of perspective and emotion informs their referential understanding.
    Graham SA; San Juan V; Khu M
    J Child Lang; 2017 May; 44(3):500-526. PubMed ID: 27817761
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The Dynamic Differences between Self- and Other-Oriented Mental Inferences: An ERP Study on a False-Belief Task.
    Wang X; Su Y; Hong M
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2021 Oct; 21(5):960-969. PubMed ID: 33907991
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Theory-of-mind use in remitted schizophrenia patients: The role of inhibition and perspective-switching.
    Wang YG; Shi JF; Roberts DL; Jiang XY; Shen ZH; Wang YQ; Wang K
    Psychiatry Res; 2015 Sep; 229(1-2):332-9. PubMed ID: 26216168
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Reduced egocentric bias when perspective-taking compared with working from rules.
    Samuel S; Frohnwieser A; Lurz R; Clayton NS
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2020 Sep; 73(9):1368-1381. PubMed ID: 32186240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.