These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

168 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33108399)

  • 1. Using judgment bias test in pet and shelter dogs (Canis familiaris): Methodological and statistical caveats.
    Burani C; Barnard S; Wells D; Pelosi A; Valsecchi P
    PLoS One; 2020; 15(10):e0241344. PubMed ID: 33108399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. A promising novel judgement bias test to evaluate affective states in dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Burani C; Pelosi A; Valsecchi P
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Aug; 25(4):837-852. PubMed ID: 34982301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The effect of repeated testing on judgement bias in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Wilson C; Hall N; Aviles-Rosa EO; Campbell K; Arnott G; Reeve C
    Anim Cogn; 2023 Mar; 26(2):477-489. PubMed ID: 36094748
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Personality traits affecting judgement bias task performance in dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Barnard S; Wells DL; Milligan ADS; Arnott G; Hepper PG
    Sci Rep; 2018 Apr; 8(1):6660. PubMed ID: 29703989
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Canine sense and sensibility: tipping points and response latency variability as an optimism index in a canine judgement bias assessment.
    Starling MJ; Branson N; Cody D; Starling TR; McGreevy PD
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(9):e107794. PubMed ID: 25229458
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Cognitive bias and paw preference in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
    Wells DL; Hepper PG; Milligan ADS; Barnard S
    J Comp Psychol; 2017 Nov; 131(4):317-325. PubMed ID: 28517942
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Oxytocin induces positive expectations about ambivalent stimuli (cognitive bias) in dogs.
    Kis A; Hernádi A; Kanizsár O; Gácsi M; Topál J
    Horm Behav; 2015 Mar; 69():1-7. PubMed ID: 25530486
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Are lateralized and bold fish optimistic or pessimistic?
    Berlinghieri F; Rizzuto G; Kruizinga L; Riedstra B; Groothuis T; Brown C
    Anim Cogn; 2024 Jun; 27(1):42. PubMed ID: 38833197
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Increased latencies to respond in a judgment bias test are not associated with pessimistic biases in rats.
    Barker TH; Howarth GS; Whittaker AL
    Behav Processes; 2018 Jan; 146():64-66. PubMed ID: 29199110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. A Behavioural Assay to Investigate Judgment Bias in Zebrafish.
    Espigares F; Martins RR; Oliveira RF
    Bio Protoc; 2022 Feb; 12(4):e4327. PubMed ID: 35340287
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Incidental spatial memory in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
    Sluka CM; Stanko K; Campbell A; Cáceres J; Panoz-Brown D; Wheeler A; Bradley J; Allen C
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):513-521. PubMed ID: 29845456
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Assessing the emotional states of dairy cows housed with or without their calves.
    Neave HW; Rault JL; Bateson M; Hvidtfeldt Jensen E; Bak Jensen M
    J Dairy Sci; 2024 Feb; 107(2):1085-1101. PubMed ID: 37709022
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Dogs are more pessimistic if their owners use two or more aversive training methods.
    Casey RA; Naj-Oleari M; Campbell S; Mendl M; Blackwell EJ
    Sci Rep; 2021 Sep; 11(1):19023. PubMed ID: 34561478
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Brief owner absence does not induce negative judgement bias in pet dogs.
    Müller CA; Riemer S; Rosam CM; Schößwender J; Range F; Huber L
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Sep; 15(5):1031-5. PubMed ID: 22870825
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Is there a bias in spatial maze judgment bias tests? Individual differences in subjects' novelty response can affect test results.
    Jardim V; Verjat A; Féron C; Châline N; Rödel HG
    Behav Brain Res; 2021 Jun; 407():113262. PubMed ID: 33775775
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Partial rewarding during clicker training does not improve naïve dogs' learning speed and induces a pessimistic-like affective state.
    Cimarelli G; Schoesswender J; Vitiello R; Huber L; Virányi Z
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Jan; 24(1):107-119. PubMed ID: 32897444
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. More exploratory house mice judge an ambiguous situation more negatively.
    Verjat A; Devienne P; Rödel HG; Féron C
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Jan; 24(1):53-64. PubMed ID: 32700167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Influence of owners' attachment style and personality on their dogs' (Canis familiaris) separation-related disorder.
    Konok V; Kosztolányi A; Rainer W; Mutschler B; Halsband U; Miklósi Á
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(2):e0118375. PubMed ID: 25706147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Are hungry sheep more pessimistic? The effects of food restriction on cognitive bias and the involvement of ghrelin in its regulation.
    Verbeek E; Ferguson D; Lee C
    Physiol Behav; 2014 Jan; 123():67-75. PubMed ID: 24096007
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.