These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

187 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33230238)

  • 1. Capuchin and rhesus monkeys show sunk cost effects in a psychomotor task.
    Watzek J; Brosnan SF
    Sci Rep; 2020 Nov; 10(1):20396. PubMed ID: 33230238
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. What are my chances? Closing the gap in uncertainty monitoring between rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2014 Jul; 40(3):303-16. PubMed ID: 25368870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Looking ahead? Computerized maze task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), and human children (Homo sapiens).
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE; Futch SE; Evans TA; Perdue BM
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 May; 129(2):160-73. PubMed ID: 25798793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Working and waiting for better rewards: self-control in two monkey species (Cebus apella and Macaca mulatta).
    Evans TA; Perdue BM; Parrish AE; Beran MJ
    Behav Processes; 2014 Mar; 103():236-42. PubMed ID: 24412729
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Do monkeys choose to choose?
    Perdue BM; Evans TA; Washburn DA; Rumbaugh DM; Beran MJ
    Learn Behav; 2014 Jun; 42(2):164-75. PubMed ID: 24567075
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Information seeking by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Smith JD
    Cognition; 2011 Jul; 120(1):90-105. PubMed ID: 21459372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) remember future responses in a computerized task.
    Beran MJ; Evans TA; Klein ED; Einstein GO
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2012 Jul; 38(3):233-43. PubMed ID: 22545901
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Task switching in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) during computerized categorization tasks.
    Smith TR; Beran MJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2018 Jul; 44(3):229-246. PubMed ID: 29847983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. (Ir)rational choices of humans, rhesus macaques, and capuchin monkeys in dynamic stochastic environments.
    Watzek J; Brosnan SF
    Cognition; 2018 Sep; 178():109-117. PubMed ID: 29852425
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.
    Reilly OT; Brosnan SF
    J Comp Psychol; 2024 May; 138(2):88-98. PubMed ID: 38095928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Hand preference for a bimanual task in tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).
    Westergaard GC; Suomi SJ
    J Comp Psychol; 1996 Dec; 110(4):406-11. PubMed ID: 8956511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Outcome expectancy and suboptimal risky choice in nonhuman primates.
    Smith TR; Beran MJ
    Learn Behav; 2020 Sep; 48(3):301-321. PubMed ID: 31997252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Conceptual thresholds for same and different in old-(Macaca mulatta) and new-world (Cebus apella) monkeys.
    Flemming TM
    Behav Processes; 2011 Mar; 86(3):316-22. PubMed ID: 21238555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Irrational choice behavior in human and nonhuman primates.
    Perdue BM; Brown ER
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Mar; 21(2):227-234. PubMed ID: 29294199
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modulate their use of an uncertainty response depending on risk.
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Church BA; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2016 Jan; 42(1):32-43. PubMed ID: 26551351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Self-control assessments of capuchin monkeys with the rotating tray task and the accumulation task.
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Rossettie MS; James BT; Whitham W; Walker B; Futch SE; Parrish AE
    Behav Processes; 2016 Aug; 129():68-79. PubMed ID: 27298233
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The psychological organization of "uncertainty" responses and "middle" responses: a dissociation in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Smith JD; Coutinho MV; Couchman JJ; Boomer J
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Jul; 35(3):371-81. PubMed ID: 19594282
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) let lesser rewards pass them by to get better rewards.
    Bramlett JL; Perdue BM; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Sep; 15(5):963-9. PubMed ID: 22689285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Rhesus monkeys' valuation of vocalizations during a free-choice task.
    Russ BE; Cohen YE
    PLoS One; 2009 Nov; 4(11):e7834. PubMed ID: 19924223
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Delay choice versus delay maintenance: different measures of delayed gratification in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Addessi E; Paglieri F; Beran MJ; Evans TA; Macchitella L; De Petrillo F; Focaroli V
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Nov; 127(4):392-8. PubMed ID: 23544770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.