These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

95 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33307123)

  • 21. Hand preference and performance on unimanual and bimanual tasks in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Fragaszy DM; Mitchell SR
    J Comp Psychol; 1990 Sep; 104(3):275-82. PubMed ID: 2225766
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Pointing, withholding information, and deception in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Mitchell RW; Anderson JR
    J Comp Psychol; 1997 Dec; 111(4):351-61. PubMed ID: 9419880
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Delaying gratification for food and tokens in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): when quantity is salient, symbolic stimuli do not improve performance.
    Evans TA; Beran MJ; Paglieri F; Addessi E
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jul; 15(4):539-48. PubMed ID: 22434403
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modulate their use of an uncertainty response depending on risk.
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Church BA; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2016 Jan; 42(1):32-43. PubMed ID: 26551351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Delay of gratification in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus).
    Anderson JR; Kuroshima H; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 May; 124(2):205-10. PubMed ID: 20476820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Experimental evidence for route integration and strategic planning in wild capuchin monkeys.
    Janson CH
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Jul; 10(3):341-56. PubMed ID: 17464518
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): the role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts.
    De Lillo C; Spinozzi G; Truppa V
    Behav Brain Res; 2007 Jul; 181(1):96-109. PubMed ID: 17467068
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Monkeys (Macaca mulatta and Cebus apella) track, enumerate, and compare multiple sets of moving items.
    Beran MJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2008 Jan; 34(1):63-74. PubMed ID: 18248115
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Performance in a tool-using task by common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Visalberghi E; Fragaszy DM; Savage-Rumbaugh S
    J Comp Psychol; 1995 Mar; 109(1):52-60. PubMed ID: 7705062
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. How capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) quantify objects and substances.
    VanMarle K; Aw J; McCrink K; Santos LR
    J Comp Psychol; 2006 Nov; 120(4):416-26. PubMed ID: 17115863
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) treat small and large numbers of items similarly during a relative quantity judgment task.
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2016 Aug; 23(4):1206-13. PubMed ID: 26689808
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Visalberghi E; Limongelli L
    J Comp Psychol; 1994 Mar; 108(1):15-22. PubMed ID: 8174341
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Strategic navigation of two-dimensional alley mazes: comparing capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees.
    Fragaszy D; Johnson-Pynn J; Hirsh E; Brakke K
    Anim Cogn; 2003 Sep; 6(3):149-60. PubMed ID: 12955584
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Individual performance across motoric self-regulation tasks are not correlated for pet dogs.
    Vernouillet AAA; Stiles LR; Andrew McCausland J; Kelly DM
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):522-536. PubMed ID: 30251102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Do squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) predict that looking leads to touching?
    Anderson JR; Kuroshima H; Kuwahata H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jul; 7(3):185-92. PubMed ID: 15022054
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Exploring whether nonhuman primates show a bias to overestimate dense quantities.
    Parrish AE; James BT; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2017 Feb; 131(1):59-68. PubMed ID: 28182487
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. I know you are not looking at me: capuchin monkeys' (Cebus apella) sensitivity to human attentional states.
    Hattori Y; Kuroshima H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):141-8. PubMed ID: 16944232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Assessment of Problem-Solving Skills and Inhibitory Control in Mice Using Water Escape Detour Test.
    Juszczak GR; Bobrowska A
    Curr Protoc Mouse Biol; 2020 Sep; 10(3):e82. PubMed ID: 32870595
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Does presentation format influence visual size discrimination in tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.)?
    Truppa V; Carducci P; Trapanese C; Hanus D
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(4):e0126001. PubMed ID: 25927363
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Analogical reasoning in a capuchin monkey (Cebus apella).
    Kennedy EH; Fragaszy DM
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):167-75. PubMed ID: 18489232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 5.