These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

233 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33423626)

  • 21. Conformist social learning leads to self-organised prevention against adverse bias in risky decision making.
    Toyokawa W; Gaissmaier W
    Elife; 2022 May; 11():. PubMed ID: 35535494
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Risky business: the neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty.
    Platt ML; Huettel SA
    Nat Neurosci; 2008 Apr; 11(4):398-403. PubMed ID: 18368046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Conditional valuation for combinations of goods in primates.
    Chung HK; Alós-Ferrer C; Tobler PN
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190669. PubMed ID: 33423622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Probability Distortion Depends on Choice Sequence in Rhesus Monkeys.
    Ferrari-Toniolo S; Bujold PM; Schultz W
    J Neurosci; 2019 Apr; 39(15):2915-2929. PubMed ID: 30705103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. What behaviour in economic games tells us about the evolution of non-human species' economic decision-making behaviour.
    Brosnan SF
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190670. PubMed ID: 33423638
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Cells, circuits, and choices: social influences on perceptual decision making.
    Mojzisch A; Krug K
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2008 Dec; 8(4):498-508. PubMed ID: 19033244
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Assessing evidence accumulation and rule learning in humans with an online game.
    Do Q; Li Y; Kane GA; McGuire JT; Scott BB
    J Neurophysiol; 2023 Jan; 129(1):131-143. PubMed ID: 36475830
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. The role of memory in distinguishing risky decisions from experience and description.
    Madan CR; Ludvig EA; Spetch ML
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2017 Oct; 70(10):2048-2059. PubMed ID: 27602887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Adaptive History Biases Result from Confidence-Weighted Accumulation of past Choices.
    Braun A; Urai AE; Donner TH
    J Neurosci; 2018 Mar; 38(10):2418-2429. PubMed ID: 29371318
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Decision neuroscience: neuroeconomics.
    Smith DV; Huettel SA
    Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci; 2010 Nov; 1(6):854-871. PubMed ID: 22754602
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Adaptable history biases in human perceptual decisions.
    Abrahamyan A; Silva LL; Dakin SC; Carandini M; Gardner JL
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2016 Jun; 113(25):E3548-57. PubMed ID: 27330086
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. A meta-analytic review of two modes of learning and the description-experience gap.
    Wulff DU; Mergenthaler-Canseco M; Hertwig R
    Psychol Bull; 2018 Feb; 144(2):140-176. PubMed ID: 29239630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Choosing the greater of two goods: neural currencies for valuation and decision making.
    Sugrue LP; Corrado GS; Newsome WT
    Nat Rev Neurosci; 2005 May; 6(5):363-75. PubMed ID: 15832198
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The long and short of it: closing the description-experience "gap" by taking the long-run view.
    Camilleri AR; Newell BR
    Cognition; 2013 Jan; 126(1):54-71. PubMed ID: 23021857
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Neuroeconomics: a view from neuroscience.
    Montague PR
    Funct Neurol; 2007; 22(4):219-34. PubMed ID: 18182129
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Reward and decision processes in the brains of humans and nonhuman primates.
    Sirigu A; Duhamel JR
    Dialogues Clin Neurosci; 2016 Mar; 18(1):45-53. PubMed ID: 27069379
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. [Risk-taking in adolescence: A neuroeconomics approach].
    Barbalat G; Domenech P; Vernet M; Fourneret P
    Encephale; 2010 Apr; 36(2):147-54. PubMed ID: 20434632
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. The construct-behavior gap and the description-experience gap: Comment on Regenwetter and Robinson (2017).
    Hertwig R; Pleskac TJ
    Psychol Rev; 2018 Oct; 125(5):844-849. PubMed ID: 30299143
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Comparative approaches to studying strategy: towards an evolutionary account of primate decision making.
    Brosnan SF; Beran MJ; Parrish AE; Price SA; Wilson BJ
    Evol Psychol; 2013 Jul; 11(3):606-27. PubMed ID: 23864296
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Of black swans and tossed coins: is the description-experience gap in risky choice limited to rare events?
    Ludvig EA; Spetch ML
    PLoS One; 2011; 6(6):e20262. PubMed ID: 21673807
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 12.