These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

144 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33423627)

  • 1. The adaptive value of probability distortion and risk-seeking in macaques' decision-making.
    Nioche A; Rougier NP; Deffains M; Bourgeois-Gironde S; Ballesta S; Boraud T
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190668. PubMed ID: 33423627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. On the Flexibility of Basic Risk Attitudes in Monkeys.
    Farashahi S; Azab H; Hayden B; Soltani A
    J Neurosci; 2018 May; 38(18):4383-4398. PubMed ID: 29626169
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. An asymmetry of treatment between lotteries involving gains and losses in rhesus monkeys.
    Nioche A; Bourgeois-Gironde S; Boraud T
    Sci Rep; 2019 Jul; 9(1):10441. PubMed ID: 31320725
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Non-human primates use combined rules when deciding under ambiguity.
    Romain A; Broihanne MH; De Marco A; Ngoubangoye B; Call J; Rebout N; Dufour V
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190672. PubMed ID: 33423632
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Probability Distortion Depends on Choice Sequence in Rhesus Monkeys.
    Ferrari-Toniolo S; Bujold PM; Schultz W
    J Neurosci; 2019 Apr; 39(15):2915-2929. PubMed ID: 30705103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Impact of internal and external factors on prosocial choices in rhesus macaques.
    Sallet J; Emberton A; Wood J; Rushworth M
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190678. PubMed ID: 33423628
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Risk-seeking for losses is associated with 5-HTTLPR, but not with transient changes in 5-HT levels.
    Neukam PT; Kroemer NB; Deza Araujo YI; Hellrung L; Pooseh S; Rietschel M; Witt SH; Schwarzenbolz U; Henle T; Smolka MN
    Psychopharmacology (Berl); 2018 Jul; 235(7):2151-2165. PubMed ID: 29730700
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Primate anterior insular cortex represents economic decision variables proposed by prospect theory.
    Yang YP; Li X; Stuphorn V
    Nat Commun; 2022 Feb; 13(1):717. PubMed ID: 35132070
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Decisions under risk in Parkinson's disease: preserved evaluation of probability and magnitude.
    Sharp ME; Viswanathan J; McKeown MJ; Appel-Cresswell S; Stoessl AJ; Barton JJ
    Neuropsychologia; 2013 Nov; 51(13):2679-89. PubMed ID: 23954375
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. What behaviour in economic games tells us about the evolution of non-human species' economic decision-making behaviour.
    Brosnan SF
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190670. PubMed ID: 33423638
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Dynamic prospect theory: Two core decision theories coexist in the gambling behavior of monkeys and humans.
    Tymula A; Wang X; Imaizumi Y; Kawai T; Kunimatsu J; Matsumoto M; Yamada H
    Sci Adv; 2023 May; 9(20):eade7972. PubMed ID: 37205752
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Monkeys (Sapajus apella and Macaca tonkeana) and great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes) play for the highest bid.
    Broihanne MH; Romain A; Call J; Thierry B; Wascher CAF; De Marco A; Verrier D; Dufour V
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 Aug; 133(3):301-312. PubMed ID: 30589294
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Economic behaviours among non-human primates.
    Bourgeois-Gironde S; Addessi E; Boraud T
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190676. PubMed ID: 33423625
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Non-human primate token use shows possibilities but also limitations for establishing a form of currency.
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190675. PubMed ID: 33423633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Neural response to reward anticipation under risk is nonlinear in probabilities.
    Hsu M; Krajbich I; Zhao C; Camerer CF
    J Neurosci; 2009 Feb; 29(7):2231-7. PubMed ID: 19228976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Reduced risk-seeking in chimpanzees in a zero-outcome game.
    Keupp S; Grueneisen S; Ludvig EA; Warneken F; Melis AP
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190673. PubMed ID: 33423631
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Prospects behind bars: analyzing decisions under risk in a prison population.
    Pachur T; Hanoch Y; Gummerum M
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2010 Oct; 17(5):630-6. PubMed ID: 21037159
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaques.
    Leca JB; Gunst N; Gardiner M; Wandia IN
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190677. PubMed ID: 33423623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Neurons in primate prefrontal cortex signal valuable social information during natural viewing.
    Adams GK; Ong WS; Pearson JM; Watson KK; Platt ML
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190666. PubMed ID: 33423624
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Monkeys are curious about counterfactual outcomes.
    Wang MZ; Hayden BY
    Cognition; 2019 Aug; 189():1-10. PubMed ID: 30889493
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.