These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

205 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33423632)

  • 1. Non-human primates use combined rules when deciding under ambiguity.
    Romain A; Broihanne MH; De Marco A; Ngoubangoye B; Call J; Rebout N; Dufour V
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190672. PubMed ID: 33423632
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Do tufted capuchin monkeys play the odds? Flexible risk preferences in Sapajus spp.
    De Petrillo F; Ventricelli M; Ponsi G; Addessi E
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):119-30. PubMed ID: 24993065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Chimpanzees and bonobos distinguish between risk and ambiguity.
    Rosati AG; Hare B
    Biol Lett; 2011 Feb; 7(1):15-8. PubMed ID: 21106573
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Non-human primate token use shows possibilities but also limitations for establishing a form of currency.
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190675. PubMed ID: 33423633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Ecological rationality: Convergent decision-making in apes and capuchins.
    De Petrillo F; Rosati AG
    Behav Processes; 2019 Jul; 164():201-213. PubMed ID: 31082478
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Emotion and decision-making under uncertainty: Physiological arousal predicts increased gambling during ambiguity but not risk.
    FeldmanHall O; Glimcher P; Baker AL; Phelps EA
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2016 Oct; 145(10):1255-1262. PubMed ID: 27690508
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Monkeys (Sapajus apella and Macaca tonkeana) and great apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii, Pan paniscus, and Pan troglodytes) play for the highest bid.
    Broihanne MH; Romain A; Call J; Thierry B; Wascher CAF; De Marco A; Verrier D; Dufour V
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 Aug; 133(3):301-312. PubMed ID: 30589294
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Gambling primates: reactions to a modified Iowa Gambling Task in humans, chimpanzees and capuchin monkeys.
    Proctor D; Williamson RA; Latzman RD; de Waal FB; Brosnan SF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Jul; 17(4):983-95. PubMed ID: 24504555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Comfort for uncertainty in pathological gamblers: a fMRI study.
    Brevers D; Bechara A; Hermoye L; Divano L; Kornreich C; Verbanck P; Noël X
    Behav Brain Res; 2015 Feb; 278():262-70. PubMed ID: 25277841
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The adaptive value of probability distortion and risk-seeking in macaques' decision-making.
    Nioche A; Rougier NP; Deffains M; Bourgeois-Gironde S; Ballesta S; Boraud T
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190668. PubMed ID: 33423627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Gambling for Gatorade: risk-sensitive decision making for fluid rewards in humans.
    Hayden BY; Platt ML
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):201-7. PubMed ID: 18719953
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Outcome expectancy and suboptimal risky choice in nonhuman primates.
    Smith TR; Beran MJ
    Learn Behav; 2020 Sep; 48(3):301-321. PubMed ID: 31997252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Body in the face of uncertainty: The role of autonomic arousal and interoception in decision-making under risk and ambiguity.
    Herman AM; Esposito G; Tsakiris M
    Psychophysiology; 2021 Aug; 58(8):e13840. PubMed ID: 33977533
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Great apes' risk-taking strategies in a decision making task.
    Haun DB; Nawroth C; Call J
    PLoS One; 2011; 6(12):e28801. PubMed ID: 22216113
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Do apes know that they could be wrong?
    Call J
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Sep; 13(5):689-700. PubMed ID: 20306102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Social inhibition and behavioural flexibility when the context changes: a comparison across six primate species.
    Amici F; Call J; Watzek J; Brosnan S; Aureli F
    Sci Rep; 2018 Feb; 8(1):3067. PubMed ID: 29449670
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Social modulation of risky decision-making in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.).
    Zoratto F; Oddi G; Gori E; Micucci A; De Petrillo F; Paglieri F; Adriani W; Laviola G; Addessi E
    Behav Brain Res; 2018 Jul; 347():37-48. PubMed ID: 29486266
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Chimpanzees, bonobos and children successfully coordinate in conflict situations.
    Sánchez-Amaro A; Duguid S; Call J; Tomasello M
    Proc Biol Sci; 2017 Jun; 284(1856):. PubMed ID: 28592680
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Ambiguity aversion in rhesus macaques.
    Hayden BY; Heilbronner SR; Platt ML
    Front Neurosci; 2010; 4():. PubMed ID: 20922060
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Variation in primate decision-making under uncertainty and the roots of human economic behaviour.
    De Petrillo F; Rosati AG
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190671. PubMed ID: 33423637
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.