These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

115 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33430535)

  • 1. Infants' Evolving Representations of Object Motion During Occlusion: A Longitudinal Study of 6- to 12-Month-Old Infants.
    Gredebäck G; von Hofsten C
    Infancy; 2004 Sep; 6(2):165-184. PubMed ID: 33430535
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Infants' representations of three-dimensional occluded objects.
    Woods RJ; Wilcox T; Armstrong J; Alexander G
    Infant Behav Dev; 2010 Dec; 33(4):663-71. PubMed ID: 20926138
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Predictive tracking over occlusions by 4-month-old infants.
    von Hofsten C; Kochukhova O; Rosander K
    Dev Sci; 2007 Sep; 10(5):625-40. PubMed ID: 17683347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. A memory span of one? Object identification in 6.5-month-old infants.
    Káldy Z; Leslie AM
    Cognition; 2005 Sep; 97(2):153-77. PubMed ID: 16226561
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Six-Month-Old Infants Predict Agents' Goal-Directed Actions on Occluded Objects.
    Applin JB; Kibbe MM
    Infancy; 2019 May; 24(3):392-410. PubMed ID: 32677190
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Two-year-olds succeed at MIT: Multiple identity tracking in 20- and 25-month-old infants.
    Cheng C; Kaldy Z; Blaser E
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Nov; 187():104649. PubMed ID: 31352226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Infants' emerging ability to represent occluded object motion.
    Rosander K; von Hofsten C
    Cognition; 2004 Feb; 91(1):1-22. PubMed ID: 14711489
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Taking an action perspective on infant's object representations.
    Gredebäck G; von Hofsten C
    Prog Brain Res; 2007; 164():265-82. PubMed ID: 17920437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Development of object concepts in infancy: Evidence for early learning in an eye-tracking paradigm.
    Johnson SP; Amso D; Slemmer JA
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2003 Sep; 100(18):10568-73. PubMed ID: 12939406
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Anticipatory reaching of seven- to eleven-month-old infants in occlusion situations.
    van Wermeskerken M; van der Kamp J; Te Velde AF; Valero-Garcia AV; Hoozemans MJ; Savelsbergh GJ
    Infant Behav Dev; 2011 Feb; 34(1):45-54. PubMed ID: 21036402
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Occlusion is hard: Comparing predictive reaching for visible and hidden objects in infants and adults.
    Hespos S; Gredebäck G; von Hofsten C; Spelke ES
    Cogn Sci; 2009 Nov; 33(8):1483-1502. PubMed ID: 20111668
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. 2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded.
    Aguiar A; Baillargeon R
    Cogn Psychol; 1999 Sep; 39(2):116-57. PubMed ID: 10462457
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Limits of Object Persistence: Young Infants Perceive Continuity of Vertical and Horizontal Trajectories, But Not 45-Degree Oblique Trajectories.
    Bremner JG; Slater AM; Mason UC; Spring J; Johnson SP
    Infancy; 2017 May; 22(3):303-322. PubMed ID: 33158355
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Interrupting infants' persisting object representations: an object-based limit?
    Cheries EW; Wynn K; Scholl BJ
    Dev Sci; 2006 Sep; 9(5):F50-8. PubMed ID: 16911437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Head and eye movements affect object processing in 4-month-old infants more than an artificial orientation cue.
    Wahl S; Michel C; Pauen S; Hoehl S
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2013 Jun; 31(Pt 2):212-30. PubMed ID: 23659892
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The relation between crawling and 9-month-old infants' visual prediction abilities in spatial object processing.
    Kubicek C; Jovanovic B; Schwarzer G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jun; 158():64-76. PubMed ID: 28236718
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Infants' perception of object trajectories.
    Johnson SP; Bremner JG; Slater A; Mason U; Foster K; Cheshire A
    Child Dev; 2003; 74(1):94-108. PubMed ID: 12625438
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Learning about occlusion: initial assumptions and rapid adjustments.
    Kochukhova O; Gredebäck G
    Cognition; 2007 Oct; 105(1):26-46. PubMed ID: 17026976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The Role of Common Ground on Object Use in Shaping the Function of Infants' Social Gaze.
    Dimitrova N
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():619. PubMed ID: 32373014
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.