These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

133 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33438835)

  • 1. Infants' learning of non-adjacent regularities from visual sequences.
    Bettoni R; Bulf H; Brady S; Johnson SP
    Infancy; 2021 Mar; 26(2):319-326. PubMed ID: 33438835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Space modulates cross-domain transfer of abstract rules in infants.
    Bulf H; Capparini C; Nava E; de Hevia MD; Macchi Cassia V
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Jan; 213():105270. PubMed ID: 34487976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Infants' statistical learning: 2- and 5-month-olds' segmentation of continuous visual sequences.
    Slone LK; Johnson SP
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 May; 133():47-56. PubMed ID: 25757016
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Violation of non-adjacent rule dependencies elicits greater attention to a talker's mouth in 15-month-old infants.
    Birulés J; Martinez-Alvarez A; Lewkowicz DJ; de Diego-Balaguer R; Pons F
    Infancy; 2022 Sep; 27(5):963-971. PubMed ID: 35833310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Audiovisual speech perception in infancy: The influence of vowel identity and infants' productive abilities on sensitivity to (mis)matches between auditory and visual speech cues.
    Altvater-Mackensen N; Mani N; Grossmann T
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Feb; 52(2):191-204. PubMed ID: 26595352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Learning to match auditory and visual speech cues: social influences on acquisition of phonological categories.
    Altvater-Mackensen N; Grossmann T
    Child Dev; 2015; 86(2):362-78. PubMed ID: 25403424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Individual differences in non-adjacent statistical dependency learning in infants.
    Lany J; Shoaib A
    J Child Lang; 2020 Mar; 47(2):483-507. PubMed ID: 31190666
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Constraints on infants' ability to extract non-adjacent dependencies from vowels and consonants.
    Weyers I; Männel C; Mueller JL
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2022 Oct; 57():101149. PubMed ID: 36084447
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Individual differences in auditory perception predict learning of non-adjacent tone sequences in 3-year-olds.
    Mueller JL; Weyers I; Friederici AD; Männel C
    Front Hum Neurosci; 2024; 18():1358380. PubMed ID: 38638804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Infants' Learning of Rule-Based Visual Sequences Predicts Language Outcome at 2 Years.
    Bettoni R; Riva V; Cantiani C; Molteni M; Macchi Cassia V; Bulf H
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():281. PubMed ID: 32158415
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Specificity of representations in infants' visual statistical learning.
    Antovich DM; Gluck SC; Goldman EJ; Graf Estes K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 May; 193():104772. PubMed ID: 32062162
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Prosodic cues enhance infants' sensitivity to nonadjacent regularities.
    Martinez-Alvarez A; Gervain J; Koulaguina E; Pons F; de Diego-Balaguer R
    Sci Adv; 2023 Apr; 9(15):eade4083. PubMed ID: 37043570
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Learning and generalizing non-adjacent dependencies in 18-month-olds: A mechanism for language acquisition?
    Grama I; Wijnen F
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(10):e0204481. PubMed ID: 30307956
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Auditory-visual speech integration by prelinguistic infants: perception of an emergent consonant in the McGurk effect.
    Burnham D; Dodd B
    Dev Psychobiol; 2004 Dec; 45(4):204-20. PubMed ID: 15549685
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Finding phrases: On the role of co-verbal facial information in learning word order in infancy.
    de la Cruz-Pavía I; Gervain J; Vatikiotis-Bateson E; Werker JF
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(11):e0224786. PubMed ID: 31710615
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Behavioral and Imaging Studies of Infant Artificial Grammar Learning.
    Gervain J; de la Cruz-Pavía I; Gerken L
    Top Cogn Sci; 2020 Jul; 12(3):815-827. PubMed ID: 30554481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Watch and listen - A cross-cultural study of audio-visual-matching behavior in 4.5-month-old infants in German and Swedish talking faces.
    Dorn K; Weinert S; Falck-Ytter T
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Aug; 52():121-129. PubMed ID: 30007216
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Twelve-month-old infants benefit from prior experience in statistical learning.
    Lany J; Gómez RL
    Psychol Sci; 2008 Dec; 19(12):1247-52. PubMed ID: 19121132
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The effect of listening experience on the discrimination of /ba/ and /pa/ in Hebrew-learning and Arabic-learning infants.
    Segal O; Hejli-Assi S; Kishon-Rabin L
    Infant Behav Dev; 2016 Feb; 42():86-99. PubMed ID: 26708235
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.