These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

220 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33505022)

  • 21. Folk explanations of behavior: a specialized use of a domain-general mechanism.
    Spunt RP; Adolphs R
    Psychol Sci; 2015 Jun; 26(6):724-36. PubMed ID: 25911123
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Neural correlates of theory-of-mind are associated with variation in children's everyday social cognition.
    Mukerji CE; Lincoln SH; Dodell-Feder D; Nelson CA; Hooker CI
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2019 Aug; 14(6):579-589. PubMed ID: 31194250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Social error monitoring in macaque frontal cortex.
    Yoshida K; Saito N; Iriki A; Isoda M
    Nat Neurosci; 2012 Sep; 15(9):1307-12. PubMed ID: 22864610
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. How the brain predicts people's behavior in relation to rules and desires. Evidence of a medio-prefrontal dissociation.
    Corradi-Dell'Acqua C; Turri F; Kaufmann L; Clément F; Schwartz S
    Cortex; 2015 Sep; 70():21-34. PubMed ID: 25820129
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Specialized mechanisms for theory of mind: are mental representations special because they are mental or because they are representations?
    Cohen AS; Sasaki JY; German TC
    Cognition; 2015 Mar; 136():49-63. PubMed ID: 25490129
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Two systems for thinking about others' thoughts in the developing brain.
    Grosse Wiesmann C; Friederici AD; Singer T; Steinbeis N
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2020 Mar; 117(12):6928-6935. PubMed ID: 32152111
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. False belief and counterfactual reasoning in a social environment.
    Van Hoeck N; Begtas E; Steen J; Kestemont J; Vandekerckhove M; Van Overwalle F
    Neuroimage; 2014 Apr; 90():315-25. PubMed ID: 24384151
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. The role of interoceptive inference in theory of mind.
    Ondobaka S; Kilner J; Friston K
    Brain Cogn; 2017 Mar; 112():64-68. PubMed ID: 26275633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Visibly constraining an agent modulates observers' automatic false-belief tracking.
    Low J; Edwards K; Butterfill SA
    Sci Rep; 2020 Jul; 10(1):11311. PubMed ID: 32647240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Can infants adopt underspecified contents into attributed beliefs? Representational prerequisites of theory of mind.
    Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Csibra G
    Cognition; 2021 Aug; 213():104640. PubMed ID: 33757642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. On Direct Social Perception.
    Spaulding S
    Conscious Cogn; 2015 Nov; 36():472-82. PubMed ID: 25623170
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning.
    Van Overwalle F
    Neuroimage; 2011 Jan; 54(2):1589-99. PubMed ID: 20869452
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Neural mechanisms of belief inference during cooperative games.
    Yoshida W; Seymour B; Friston KJ; Dolan RJ
    J Neurosci; 2010 Aug; 30(32):10744-51. PubMed ID: 20702705
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Social-cognitive deficits in normal aging.
    Moran JM; Jolly E; Mitchell JP
    J Neurosci; 2012 Apr; 32(16):5553-61. PubMed ID: 22514317
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Individual differences in response of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex predict daily social behavior.
    Powers KE; Chavez RS; Heatherton TF
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2016 Jan; 11(1):121-6. PubMed ID: 26206505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Thinking about the thoughts of others; temporal and spatial neural activation during false belief reasoning.
    Mossad SI; AuCoin-Power M; Urbain C; Smith ML; Pang EW; Taylor MJ
    Neuroimage; 2016 Jul; 134():320-327. PubMed ID: 27039146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Monkeys represent others' knowledge but not their beliefs.
    Marticorena DC; Ruiz AM; Mukerji C; Goddu A; Santos LR
    Dev Sci; 2011 Nov; 14(6):1406-16. PubMed ID: 22010899
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Macaques Exhibit Implicit Gaze Bias Anticipating Others' False-Belief-Driven Actions via Medial Prefrontal Cortex.
    Hayashi T; Akikawa R; Kawasaki K; Egawa J; Minamimoto T; Kobayashi K; Kato S; Hori Y; Nagai Y; Iijima A; Someya T; Hasegawa I
    Cell Rep; 2020 Mar; 30(13):4433-4444.e5. PubMed ID: 32234478
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Dissociation of understanding from applying others' false beliefs in remitted schizophrenia: evidence from a computerized referential communication task.
    Wang YG; Roberts DL; Xu BH
    BMC Psychiatry; 2013 May; 13():141. PubMed ID: 23683146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size.
    Lewis PA; Rezaie R; Brown R; Roberts N; Dunbar RI
    Neuroimage; 2011 Aug; 57(4):1624-9. PubMed ID: 21616156
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.