These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

146 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33566180)

  • 1. The effects of distance on pointing comprehension in shelter dogs.
    Lyn H; Broadway M; Jett SE; Samuelson MM; Christopher J; Chenkin B
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Jul; 24(4):855-865. PubMed ID: 33566180
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Reassessing shelter dogs' use of human communicative cues in the standard object-choice task.
    Osborne T; Mulcahy NJ
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213166. PubMed ID: 30845205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. What's the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand human pointing gestures.
    D'Aniello B; Alterisio A; Scandurra A; Petremolo E; Iommelli MR; Aria M
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Jul; 20(4):777-787. PubMed ID: 28508126
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Virányi Z; Gácsi M; Kubinyi E; Topál J; Belényi B; Ujfalussy D; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jul; 11(3):373-87. PubMed ID: 18183437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.
    Kirchhofer KC; Zimmermann F; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(2):e30913. PubMed ID: 22347411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Comprehension and utilisation of pointing gestures and gazing in dog-human communication in relatively complex situations.
    Lakatos G; Gácsi M; Topál J; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):201-13. PubMed ID: 21927851
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs.
    Gácsi M; Kara E; Belényi B; Topál J; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2009 May; 12(3):471-9. PubMed ID: 19130102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. What or where? The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Tauzin T; Csík A; Kis A; Topál J
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Nov; 129(4):334-8. PubMed ID: 26147704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. A comparative approach to dogs' (Canis familiaris) and human infants' comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures.
    Lakatos G; Soproni K; Dóka A; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jul; 12(4):621-31. PubMed ID: 19343382
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Comprehension of the communicative intent behind pointing and gazing gestures by young children with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome.
    John AE; Mervis CB
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2010 Aug; 53(4):950-60. PubMed ID: 20605941
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Test sensitivity is important for detecting variability in pointing comprehension in canines.
    Pongrácz P; Gácsi M; Hegedüs D; Péter A; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Sep; 16(5):721-35. PubMed ID: 23392852
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills.
    Gácsi M; Györi B; Virányi Z; Kubinyi E; Range F; Belényi B; Miklósi A
    PLoS One; 2009 Aug; 4(8):e6584. PubMed ID: 19714197
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Selective responding to human ostensive communication is an early developing capacity of domestic dogs.
    Byosiere SE; Mundry R; Range F; Virányi Z
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13361. PubMed ID: 36545915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Eighteen-month-old human infants show intensive development in comprehension of different types of pointing gestures.
    Pfandler E; Lakatos G; Miklósi Á
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Sep; 16(5):711-9. PubMed ID: 23412666
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Dogs outperform cats both in their testability and relying on human pointing gestures: a comparative study.
    Salamon A; Uccheddu S; Csepregi M; Miklósi Á; Gácsi M
    Sci Rep; 2023 Oct; 13(1):17837. PubMed ID: 37857683
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Getting away from the point: the emergence of ostensive gestures and their functions.
    Moreno-Núñez A; Rodríguez C; Miranda-Zapata E
    J Child Lang; 2020 May; 47(3):556-578. PubMed ID: 31685056
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. What experimental experience affects dogs' comprehension of human communicative actions?
    Hauser MD; Comins JA; Pytka LM; Cahill DP; Velez-Calderon S
    Behav Processes; 2011 Jan; 86(1):7-20. PubMed ID: 20696218
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans.
    Miklósi A; Pongrácz P; Lakatos G; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 May; 119(2):179-86. PubMed ID: 15982161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Horses with sustained attention follow the pointing of a human who knows where food is hidden.
    Ringhofer M; Trösch M; Lansade L; Yamamoto S
    Sci Rep; 2021 Aug; 11(1):16184. PubMed ID: 34376761
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.