These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
108 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33636635)
1. Children understand communication intuitively, but indirect communication makes them think twice-Evidence from pupillometry and looking patterns. Schulze C; Buttelmann D J Exp Child Psychol; 2021 Jun; 206():105105. PubMed ID: 33636635 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Pupillometry reveals communication-induced object expectations in 12- but not 8-month-old infants. Pätzold W; Liszkowski U Dev Sci; 2019 Nov; 22(6):e12832. PubMed ID: 30942933 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. 3-year-old children make relevance inferences in indirect verbal communication. Schulze C; Grassmann S; Tomasello M Child Dev; 2013; 84(6):2079-93. PubMed ID: 23550944 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Five-year-olds' sensitivity to knowledge discrepancies about object identity during online language comprehension. Ju N; San Juan V; Chambers CG; Graham SA J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Dec; 236():105745. PubMed ID: 37523788 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. An eye-tracking investigation of the cognitive processes involved in the comprehension of simple and complex communicative acts. Parola A; Bosco FM Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2022 Oct; 75(10):1976-1995. PubMed ID: 35084282 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. The cognitive demands of remembering a speaker's perspective and managing common ground size modulate 8- and 10-year-olds' perspective-taking abilities. Zhao L; Wang JJ; Apperly IA J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Oct; 174():130-149. PubMed ID: 29940398 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Comprehension of indirect answers: Developmental trajectory for preschool- and early elementary school-aged children with typical development. Huang T; Finestack LH J Child Lang; 2024 May; 51(3):526-549. PubMed ID: 37644915 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development. Chouinard MM Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Social eye gaze modulates processing of speech and co-speech gesture. Holler J; Schubotz L; Kelly S; Hagoort P; Schuetze M; Özyürek A Cognition; 2014 Dec; 133(3):692-7. PubMed ID: 25255036 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. The relations between children's communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Nilsen ES; Graham SA Cogn Psychol; 2009 Mar; 58(2):220-49. PubMed ID: 18809176 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Young children use shared experience to interpret definite reference. Schmerse D; Lieven E; Tomasello M J Child Lang; 2015 Sep; 42(5):1146-57. PubMed ID: 25275347 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Two-year-olds exclude novel objects as potential referents of novel words based on pragmatics. Grassmann S; Stracke M; Tomasello M Cognition; 2009 Sep; 112(3):488-93. PubMed ID: 19616205 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Preschool children's interpretation of object-initial sentences: neural correlates of their behavioral performance. Schipke CS; Knoll LJ; Friederici AD; Oberecker R Dev Sci; 2012 Nov; 15(6):762-74. PubMed ID: 23106730 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Young children's understanding of markedness in non-verbal communication. Liebal K; Carpenter M; Tomasello M J Child Lang; 2011 Sep; 38(4):888-903. PubMed ID: 21382221 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. The health and well-being of caregivers of children with cerebral palsy. Raina P; O'Donnell M; Rosenbaum P; Brehaut J; Walter SD; Russell D; Swinton M; Zhu B; Wood E Pediatrics; 2005 Jun; 115(6):e626-36. PubMed ID: 15930188 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Do executive function and theory of mind predict pragmatic abilities following traumatic brain injury? An analysis of sincere, deceitful and ironic communicative acts. Bosco FM; Gabbatore I; Angeleri R; Zettin M; Parola A J Commun Disord; 2018; 75():102-117. PubMed ID: 29887277 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Four-year-olds incorporate speaker knowledge into pragmatic inferences. Kampa A; Papafragou A Dev Sci; 2020 May; 23(3):e12920. PubMed ID: 31693776 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. The object of my desire: Five-year-olds rapidly reason about a speaker's desire during referential communication. San Juan V; Chambers CG; Berman J; Humphry C; Graham SA J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Oct; 162():101-119. PubMed ID: 28600922 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Sincere, Deceitful, and Ironic Communicative Acts and the Role of the Theory of Mind in Childhood. Bosco FM; Gabbatore I Front Psychol; 2017; 8():21. PubMed ID: 28194120 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]