These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

168 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 33713876)

  • 1. Inhibiting intuition: Scaffolding children's theory construction about species evolution in the face of competing explanations.
    Ronfard S; Brown S; Doncaster E; Kelemen D
    Cognition; 2021 Jun; 211():104635. PubMed ID: 33713876
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Explanation and generalization in young children's strategy learning.
    Crowley K; Siegler RS
    Child Dev; 1999; 70(2):304-16. PubMed ID: 10218256
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Fuzzy-trace theory: dual processes in memory, reasoning, and cognitive neuroscience.
    Brainerd CJ; Reyna VF
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2001; 28():41-100. PubMed ID: 11605365
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Children's judgments in theory choice tasks: scientific rationality in childhood.
    Samarapungavan A
    Cognition; 1992 Oct; 45(1):1-32. PubMed ID: 1424496
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Thinking in a foreign language distorts allocation of cognitive effort: Evidence from reasoning.
    Białek M; Muda R; Stewart K; Niszczota P; Pieńkosz D
    Cognition; 2020 Dec; 205():104420. PubMed ID: 33032818
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Moral learning as intuitive theory revision.
    Rhodes M; Wellman H
    Cognition; 2017 Oct; 167():191-200. PubMed ID: 27613004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The Roles of Intuition and Informants' Expertise in Children's Epistemic Trust.
    Lane JD; Harris PL
    Child Dev; 2015; 86(3):919-26. PubMed ID: 25425347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Young children can be taught basic natural selection using a picture-storybook intervention.
    Kelemen D; Emmons NA; Seston Schillaci R; Ganea PA
    Psychol Sci; 2014 Apr; 25(4):893-902. PubMed ID: 24503874
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Promoting scientific understanding and conceptual change in young children using explanations and guidance.
    Venkadasalam VP; Larsen NE; Ganea PA
    Dev Psychol; 2024 Apr; 60(4):729-746. PubMed ID: 38358659
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Belief inhibition in children's reasoning: memory-based evidence.
    Steegen S; De Neys W
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Jun; 112(2):231-42. PubMed ID: 22402412
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Specifying the domain-general resources that contribute to conceptual construction: Evidence from the child's acquisition of vitalist biology.
    Tardiff N; Bascandziev I; Carey S; Zaitchik D
    Cognition; 2020 Feb; 195():104090. PubMed ID: 31751816
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Reasoning in middle childhood: a dynamic model of performance on transitivity tasks.
    Rabinowitz FM; Grant MJ; Howe ML; Walsh C
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1994 Oct; 58(2):252-88. PubMed ID: 7964443
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Children's intuitive mathematics: the development of knowledge about nonlinear growth.
    Ebersbach M; Wilkening F
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(1):296-308. PubMed ID: 17328706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Could it? Should it? Cognitive reflection facilitates children's reasoning about possibility and permissibility.
    Shtulman A; Harrington C; Hetzel C; Kim J; Palumbo C; Rountree-Shtulman T
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Nov; 235():105727. PubMed ID: 37385146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Development of intuitive rules: evaluating the application of the dual-system framework to understanding children's intuitive reasoning.
    Osman M; Stavy R
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2006 Dec; 13(6):935-53. PubMed ID: 17484417
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The bright homunculus in our head: Individual differences in intuitive sensitivity to logical validity.
    Ghasemi O; Handley S; Howarth S
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2022 Mar; 75(3):508-535. PubMed ID: 34427470
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The role of domain-general cognitive resources in children's construction of a vitalist theory of biology.
    Bascandziev I; Tardiff N; Zaitchik D; Carey S
    Cogn Psychol; 2018 Aug; 104():1-28. PubMed ID: 29587182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. A description-experience gap in statistical intuitions: Of smart babies, risk-savvy chimps, intuitive statisticians, and stupid grown-ups.
    Schulze C; Hertwig R
    Cognition; 2021 May; 210():104580. PubMed ID: 33667974
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The probable and the possible at 12 months: intuitive reasoning about the uncertain future.
    Cesana-Arlotti N; Téglás E; Bonatti LL
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2012; 43():1-25. PubMed ID: 23205406
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.