These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

134 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 34043285)

  • 1. Specifying links between infants' theory of mind, associative learning, and selective trust.
    Crivello C; Grossman S; Poulin-Dubois D
    Infancy; 2021 Sep; 26(5):664-685. PubMed ID: 34043285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Selective social learning in infancy: looking for mechanisms.
    Crivello C; Phillips S; Poulin-Dubois D
    Dev Sci; 2018 May; 21(3):e12592. PubMed ID: 28856760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Knowing who knows: Metacognitive and causal learning abilities guide infants' selective social learning.
    Kuzyk O; Grossman S; Poulin-Dubois D
    Dev Sci; 2020 May; 23(3):e12904. PubMed ID: 31519037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Infants' Ability to Detect Emotional Incongruency: Deep or Shallow?
    Crivello C; Poulin-Dubois D
    Infancy; 2019 Jul; 24(4):480-500. PubMed ID: 32677254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Is a Bird an Apple? The Effect of Speaker Labeling Accuracy on Infants' Word Learning, Imitation, and Helping Behaviors.
    Brooker I; Poulin-Dubois D
    Infancy; 2013 Aug; 18(Suppl 1):E46-E68. PubMed ID: 26807045
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Theory of mind selectively predicts preschoolers' knowledge-based selective word learning.
    Brosseau-Liard P; Penney D; Poulin-Dubois D
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2015 Nov; 33(4):464-75. PubMed ID: 26211504
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Do I know what they know? Linking metacognition, theory of mind, and selective social learning.
    Dutemple E; Hakimi H; Poulin-Dubois D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Mar; 227():105572. PubMed ID: 36371850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Exposure to behavioral regularities in everyday life predicts infants' acquisition of mental state vocabulary.
    Ruffman T; Chen L; Lorimer B; Vanier S; Edgar K; Scarf D; Taumoepeau M
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13343. PubMed ID: 36373496
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Theory of mind and emotion recognition skills in children with specific language impairment, autism spectrum disorder and typical development: group differences and connection to knowledge of grammatical morphology, word-finding abilities and verbal working memory.
    Loukusa S; Mäkinen L; Kuusikko-Gauffin S; Ebeling H; Moilanen I
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2014; 49(4):498-507. PubMed ID: 24888967
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Tracking speakers' false beliefs: is theory of mind available earlier for word learning?
    Houston-Price C; Goddard K; Séclier C; Grant SC; Reid CJ; Boyden LE; Williams R
    Dev Sci; 2011 Jul; 14(4):623-34. PubMed ID: 21676084
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Social cognition and trust: Exploring the role of theory of mind and hostile attribution bias in children's skepticism of inaccurate informants.
    Palmquist CM; Floersheimer A; Crum K; Ruggiero J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Mar; 215():105341. PubMed ID: 34906763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Sixteen-month-old infants are sensitive to competence in third-party observational learning.
    Lee V; Rutherford MD
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Aug; 52():114-120. PubMed ID: 30007215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Statistical learning as a basis for social understanding in children.
    Ruffman T; Taumoepeau M; Perkins C
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):87-104. PubMed ID: 22429035
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Infants' acceptance of phonotactically illegal word forms as object labels.
    Vukatana E; Curtin S; Graham SA
    J Child Lang; 2016 Nov; 43(6):1400-11. PubMed ID: 26671660
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning.
    Krogh-Jespersen S; Echols CH
    Child Dev; 2012; 83(2):581-90. PubMed ID: 22239543
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Putting Mutual Exclusivity in Context: Speaker Race Influences Monolingual and Bilingual Infants' Word-Learning Assumptions.
    Weatherhead D; Kandhadai P; Hall DG; Werker JF
    Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 92(5):1735-1751. PubMed ID: 34213010
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning.
    Durrleman S; Franck J
    J Commun Disord; 2015; 54():15-31. PubMed ID: 25637130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The effect of a looker's past reliability on infants' reasoning about beliefs.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Chow V
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1576-82. PubMed ID: 19899915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Infants' understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: Interrelations among violation of expectancy and imitation tasks.
    Olineck KM; Poulin-Dubois D
    Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Dec; 32(4):404-15. PubMed ID: 19619899
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.