These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

192 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 34091901)

  • 21. Experimental Evidence that Social Relationships Determine Individual Foraging Behavior.
    Firth JA; Voelkl B; Farine DR; Sheldon BC
    Curr Biol; 2015 Dec; 25(23):3138-43. PubMed ID: 26585280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Inhibitory control, exploration behaviour and manipulated ecological context are associated with foraging flexibility in the great tit.
    Coomes JR; Davidson GL; Reichert MS; Kulahci IG; Troisi CA; Quinn JL
    J Anim Ecol; 2022 Feb; 91(2):320-333. PubMed ID: 34693529
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Learning rules for social foragers: implications for the producer-scrounger game and ideal free distribution theory.
    Beauchamp G
    J Theor Biol; 2000 Nov; 207(1):21-35. PubMed ID: 11027477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Slow-exploring captive red knots were quicker to find food in a social setting than fast explorers.
    Roncoroni A; Ersoy S; Bijleveld AI
    Biol Lett; 2024 Jul; 20(7):20240139. PubMed ID: 39046288
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Older and wiser? Age differences in foraging and learning by an endangered passerine.
    Franks VR; Thorogood R
    Behav Processes; 2018 Mar; 148():1-9. PubMed ID: 29273549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Producers and scroungers: feeding-type composition changes with group size in a socially foraging spider.
    Dumke M; Herberstein ME; Schneider JM
    Proc Biol Sci; 2016 Apr; 283(1828):. PubMed ID: 27075253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Social learning in birds and its role in shaping a foraging niche.
    Slagsvold T; Wiebe KL
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2011 Apr; 366(1567):969-77. PubMed ID: 21357219
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Elasmobranch cognitive ability: using electroreceptive foraging behaviour to demonstrate learning, habituation and memory in a benthic shark.
    Kimber JA; Sims DW; Bellamy PH; Gill AB
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Jan; 17(1):55-65. PubMed ID: 23620366
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. LEARNING TO CHOOSE AMONG SOCIAL FORAGING STRATEGIES IN ADULT HOUSE SPARROWS (Passer domesticus).
    Belmaker A; Motro U; Feldman MW; Lotem A
    Ethology; 2012 Nov; 118(11):1111-1121. PubMed ID: 23226911
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Scrounging by foragers can resolve the paradox of enrichment.
    Toyokawa W
    R Soc Open Sci; 2017 Mar; 4(3):160830. PubMed ID: 28405371
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Personality-dependent differences in problem-solving performance in a social context reflect foraging strategies.
    Zandberg L; Quinn JL; Naguib M; van Oers K
    Behav Processes; 2017 Jan; 134():95-102. PubMed ID: 27667554
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Individual differences in the use of social information in foraging by captive great tits.
    Marchetti C; Drent PJ
    Anim Behav; 2000 Jul; 60(1):131-140. PubMed ID: 10924212
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Food-caching mountain chickadees can learn abstract rules to solve a complex spatial-temporal pattern.
    Benedict LM; Heinen VK; Welklin JF; Sonnenberg BR; Whitenack LE; Bridge ES; Pravosudov VV
    Curr Biol; 2023 Aug; 33(15):3136-3144.e5. PubMed ID: 37442137
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Manipulating actions: A selective two-option device for cognitive experiments in wild animals.
    Wild S; Alarcón-Nieto G; Chimento M; Aplin LM
    J Anim Ecol; 2023 Aug; 92(8):1509-1519. PubMed ID: 35672881
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Social learning within and across predator species reduces attacks on novel aposematic prey.
    Hämäläinen L; Mappes J; Rowland HM; Teichmann M; Thorogood R
    J Anim Ecol; 2020 May; 89(5):1153-1164. PubMed ID: 32077104
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. An arms race between producers and scroungers can drive the evolution of social cognition.
    Arbilly M; Weissman DB; Feldman MW; Grodzinski U
    Behav Ecol; 2014 May; 25(3):487-495. PubMed ID: 24822021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The effects of food limitation on behavior, corticosterone, and the use of social information in the red crossbill (Loxia curvirostra).
    Wurtz MC; Cussen V; Cornelius JM
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Nov; 24(6):1305-1317. PubMed ID: 34047864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Exploration speed in captivity predicts foraging tactics and diet in free-living red knots.
    Ersoy S; Beardsworth CE; Dekinga A; van der Meer MTJ; Piersma T; Groothuis TGG; Bijleveld AI
    J Anim Ecol; 2022 Feb; 91(2):356-366. PubMed ID: 34753198
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. [Social learning of optimal responding and producer-scrounger strategies in flocks of pigeons].
    Cabrera R; Durán A; Nieto J
    Psicothema; 2006 Nov; 18(4):724-9. PubMed ID: 17296109
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Learning the ecological niche.
    Slagsvold T; Wiebe KL
    Proc Biol Sci; 2007 Jan; 274(1606):19-23. PubMed ID: 17015332
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.