These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

152 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 34344181)

  • 41. Socially learned preferences for differentially rewarded tokens in the brown capuchin monkey (Cebus apella).
    Brosnan SF; de Waal FB
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Jun; 118(2):133-9. PubMed ID: 15250800
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.
    Reilly OT; Brosnan SF
    J Comp Psychol; 2024 May; 138(2):88-98. PubMed ID: 38095928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Touch improves visual discrimination of object features in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.).
    Carducci P; Squillace V; Manzi G; Truppa V
    Behav Processes; 2020 Mar; 172():104044. PubMed ID: 31954810
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Transport of functionally appropriate tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Judge PG; Bruno S
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Mar; 74(3):199-209. PubMed ID: 24006538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. The density bias: Capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) prefer densely arranged items in a food-choice task.
    Parrish AE; French KA; Guild AS; Creamer CL; Rossettie MS; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2020 May; 134(2):232-240. PubMed ID: 31971398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella fail to understand a cooperative task.
    Chalmeau R; Visalberghi E; Gallo A
    Anim Behav; 1997 Nov; 54(5):1215-25. PubMed ID: 9398374
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Inferences about the location of food in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) in two sensory modalities.
    Sabbatini G; Visalberghi E
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):156-66. PubMed ID: 18489231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Understanding the functional properties of tools: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) attend to tool features differently.
    Sabbatini G; Truppa V; Hribar A; Gambetta B; Call J; Visalberghi E
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jul; 15(4):577-90. PubMed ID: 22466753
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Do capuchin monkeys (
    Jordan EJ; Völter CJ; Seed AM
    Cogn Neuropsychol; 2021; 38(7-8):531-543. PubMed ID: 35732407
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Picture processing in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Truppa V; Spinozzi G; Stegagno T; Fagot J
    Behav Processes; 2009 Oct; 82(2):140-52. PubMed ID: 19501136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Identity concept learning in matching-to-sample tasks by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Truppa V; Garofoli D; Castorina G; Piano Mortari E; Natale F; Visalberghi E
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Nov; 13(6):835-48. PubMed ID: 20574839
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Delay choice versus delay maintenance: different measures of delayed gratification in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Addessi E; Paglieri F; Beran MJ; Evans TA; Macchitella L; De Petrillo F; Focaroli V
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Nov; 127(4):392-8. PubMed ID: 23544770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Outcome expectancy and suboptimal risky choice in nonhuman primates.
    Smith TR; Beran MJ
    Learn Behav; 2020 Sep; 48(3):301-321. PubMed ID: 31997252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Forelimb dimensions and goniometry of the wrist and fingers in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Developmental and comparative aspects.
    Fragaszy DM; Adams-Curtis LE; Baer JF; Carlson-Lammers R
    Am J Primatol; 1989; 17(2):133-146. PubMed ID: 31968845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Waiting for what comes later: capuchin monkeys show self-control even for nonvisible delayed rewards.
    Perdue BM; Bramlett JL; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Sep; 18(5):1105-12. PubMed ID: 26024691
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Are capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) inequity averse?
    Dubreuil D; Gentile MS; Visalberghi E
    Proc Biol Sci; 2006 May; 273(1591):1223-8. PubMed ID: 16720395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Copying without rewards: socially influenced foraging decisions among brown capuchin monkeys.
    Bonnie KE; de Waal FB
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Jul; 10(3):283-92. PubMed ID: 17929373
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Capuchin and rhesus monkeys show sunk cost effects in a psychomotor task.
    Watzek J; Brosnan SF
    Sci Rep; 2020 Nov; 10(1):20396. PubMed ID: 33230238
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? An argument for the use of sapajus and cebus.
    Alfaro JW; Silva JD; Rylands AB
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Apr; 74(4):273-86. PubMed ID: 22328205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. The hybrid delay task: can capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) sustain a delay after an initial choice to do so?
    Paglieri F; Focaroli V; Bramlett J; Tierno V; McIntyre JM; Addessi E; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    Behav Processes; 2013 Mar; 94():45-54. PubMed ID: 23274585
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.