These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

151 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 34344181)

  • 61. Copying without rewards: socially influenced foraging decisions among brown capuchin monkeys.
    Bonnie KE; de Waal FB
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Jul; 10(3):283-92. PubMed ID: 17929373
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 62. Capuchin and rhesus monkeys show sunk cost effects in a psychomotor task.
    Watzek J; Brosnan SF
    Sci Rep; 2020 Nov; 10(1):20396. PubMed ID: 33230238
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 63. How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? An argument for the use of sapajus and cebus.
    Alfaro JW; Silva JD; Rylands AB
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Apr; 74(4):273-86. PubMed ID: 22328205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 64. Navigating two-dimensional mazes: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and capuchins (Cebus apella sp.) profit from experience differently.
    Fragaszy DM; Kennedy E; Murnane A; Menzel C; Brewer G; Johnson-Pynn J; Hopkins W
    Anim Cogn; 2009 May; 12(3):491-504. PubMed ID: 19148688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 65. The hybrid delay task: can capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) sustain a delay after an initial choice to do so?
    Paglieri F; Focaroli V; Bramlett J; Tierno V; McIntyre JM; Addessi E; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    Behav Processes; 2013 Mar; 94():45-54. PubMed ID: 23274585
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 66. Use of olfactory cues in foraging by owl monkeys (Aotus nancymai) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Bolen RH; Green SM
    J Comp Psychol; 1997 Jun; 111(2):152-8. PubMed ID: 9170280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 67. Quantity judgments of sequentially presented food items by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Evans TA; Beran MJ; Harris EH; Rice DF
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):97-105. PubMed ID: 18670794
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 68. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) are sensitive to others' reward: an experimental analysis of food-choice for conspecifics.
    Takimoto A; Kuroshima H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Mar; 13(2):249-61. PubMed ID: 19609580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 69. How can I find what I want? Can children, chimpanzees and capuchin monkeys form abstract representations to guide their behavior in a sampling task?
    Felsche E; Völter CJ; Herrmann E; Seed AM; Buchsbaum D
    Cognition; 2024 Apr; 245():105721. PubMed ID: 38262272
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 70. Inference by exclusion in lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus), a hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas), capuchins (Sapajus apella), and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus).
    Marsh HL; Vining AQ; Levendoski EK; Judge PG
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Aug; 129(3):256-67. PubMed ID: 26010194
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 71. Tokens improve capuchin performance in the reverse-reward contingency task.
    Addessi E; Rossi S
    Proc Biol Sci; 2011 Mar; 278(1707):849-54. PubMed ID: 20861046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 72. Hierarchical complexity of combinatorial manipulation in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Westergaard GC; Suomi SJ
    Am J Primatol; 1994; 32(3):171-176. PubMed ID: 31936923
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 73. Solving small spaces: investigating the use of landmark cues in brown capuchins (Cebus apella).
    Hughes KD; Mullo E; Santos LR
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Sep; 16(5):803-17. PubMed ID: 23430144
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 74. Capuchin monkeys learn to use information equally well from individual exploration and social demonstration.
    Kean D; Renner E; Atkinson M; Caldwell CA
    Anim Cogn; 2023 Mar; 26(2):435-450. PubMed ID: 36064832
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 75. Experimental field study of problem-solving using tools in free-ranging capuchins (Sapajus nigritus, formerly Cebus nigritus).
    Garber PA; Gomes DF; Bicca-Marques JC
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Apr; 74(4):344-58. PubMed ID: 21538454
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 76. Capuchin monkeys (sometimes) go when they know: Confidence movements in Sapajus apella.
    Smith TR; Parrish AE; Creamer C; Rossettie M; Beran MJ
    Cognition; 2020 Jun; 199():104237. PubMed ID: 32112968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 77. Analogical reasoning in a capuchin monkey (Cebus apella).
    Kennedy EH; Fragaszy DM
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):167-75. PubMed ID: 18489232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 78. Animal behaviour: fair refusal by capuchin monkeys.
    Wynne CD
    Nature; 2004 Mar; 428(6979):140; discussion 140. PubMed ID: 15014490
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 79. Recently Integrated
    Storer JM; Walker JA; Rockwell CE; Mores G; Beckstrom TO; Orkin JD; Melin AD; Phillips KA; Roos C; Batzer MA
    Genes (Basel); 2022 Mar; 13(4):. PubMed ID: 35456378
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 80. Anticipating future actions: Motor planning improves with age in wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus).
    Truppa V; Sabbatini G; Izar P; Fragaszy DM; Visalberghi E
    Dev Sci; 2021 Jul; 24(4):e13077. PubMed ID: 33342007
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.