These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

132 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 34398884)

  • 21. Visual Hallucinations Are Characterized by Impaired Sensory Evidence Accumulation: Insights From Hierarchical Drift Diffusion Modeling in Parkinson's Disease.
    O'Callaghan C; Hall JM; Tomassini A; Muller AJ; Walpola IC; Moustafa AA; Shine JM; Lewis SJG
    Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging; 2017 Nov; 2(8):680-688. PubMed ID: 29560902
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions.
    Krajbich I; Rangel A
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2011 Aug; 108(33):13852-7. PubMed ID: 21808009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Multi-attribute, multi-alternative models of choice: Choice, reaction time, and process tracing.
    Cohen AL; Kang N; Leise TL
    Cogn Psychol; 2017 Nov; 98():45-72. PubMed ID: 28843070
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Modeling other minds: Bayesian inference explains human choices in group decision-making.
    Khalvati K; Park SA; Mirbagheri S; Philippe R; Sestito M; Dreher JC; Rao RPN
    Sci Adv; 2019 Nov; 5(11):eaax8783. PubMed ID: 31807706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. A probabilistic, distributed, recursive mechanism for decision-making in the brain.
    Caballero JA; Humphries MD; Gurney KN
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2018 Apr; 14(4):e1006033. PubMed ID: 29614077
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Pointing, looking at, and pressing keys: A diffusion model account of response modality.
    Gomez P; Ratcliff R; Childers R
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2015 Dec; 41(6):1515-23. PubMed ID: 26322685
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Optimal policy for uncertainty estimation concurrent with decision making.
    Li X; Su R; Chen Y; Yang T
    Cell Rep; 2023 Mar; 42(3):112232. PubMed ID: 36924497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. The LATER model of reaction time and decision.
    Noorani I; Carpenter RH
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2016 May; 64():229-51. PubMed ID: 26915927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Uncertainty-driven regulation of learning and exploration in adolescents: A computational account.
    Jepma M; Schaaf JV; Visser I; Huizenga HM
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2020 Sep; 16(9):e1008276. PubMed ID: 32997659
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Confidence reports in decision-making with multiple alternatives violate the Bayesian confidence hypothesis.
    Li HH; Ma WJ
    Nat Commun; 2020 Apr; 11(1):2004. PubMed ID: 32332712
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Dopaminergic Modulation of Human Intertemporal Choice: A Diffusion Model Analysis Using the D2-Receptor Antagonist Haloperidol.
    Wagner B; Clos M; Sommer T; Peters J
    J Neurosci; 2020 Oct; 40(41):7936-7948. PubMed ID: 32948675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Disentangling decision models: from independence to competition.
    Teodorescu AR; Usher M
    Psychol Rev; 2013 Jan; 120(1):1-38. PubMed ID: 23356779
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice.
    Krajbich I; Armel C; Rangel A
    Nat Neurosci; 2010 Oct; 13(10):1292-8. PubMed ID: 20835253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Learning optimal decisions with confidence.
    Drugowitsch J; Mendonça AG; Mainen ZF; Pouget A
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2019 Dec; 116(49):24872-24880. PubMed ID: 31732671
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Efficient coding of subjective value.
    PolanĂ­a R; Woodford M; Ruff CC
    Nat Neurosci; 2019 Jan; 22(1):134-142. PubMed ID: 30559477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Improving the reliability of model-based decision-making estimates in the two-stage decision task with reaction-times and drift-diffusion modeling.
    Shahar N; Hauser TU; Moutoussis M; Moran R; Keramati M; ; Dolan RJ
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2019 Feb; 15(2):e1006803. PubMed ID: 30759077
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. A Two-Stage Process Model of Sensory Discrimination: An Alternative to Drift-Diffusion.
    Sun P; Landy MS
    J Neurosci; 2016 Nov; 36(44):11259-11274. PubMed ID: 27807167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. A Probability Distribution over Latent Causes, in the Orbitofrontal Cortex.
    Chan SC; Niv Y; Norman KA
    J Neurosci; 2016 Jul; 36(30):7817-28. PubMed ID: 27466328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Optimal inference with suboptimal models: addiction and active Bayesian inference.
    Schwartenbeck P; FitzGerald TH; Mathys C; Dolan R; Wurst F; Kronbichler M; Friston K
    Med Hypotheses; 2015 Feb; 84(2):109-17. PubMed ID: 25561321
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Source preference and ambiguity aversion: models and evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging experiments.
    Chew SH; Li KK; Chark R; Zhong S
    Adv Health Econ Health Serv Res; 2008; 20():179-201. PubMed ID: 19552309
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.