These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

160 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 35020108)

  • 61. Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability.
    Persson ME; Roth LS; Johnsson M; Wright D; Jensen P
    Genes Brain Behav; 2015 Apr; 14(4):337-44. PubMed ID: 25703740
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 62. What Are the Ingredients for an Inequity Paradigm? Manipulating the Experimenter's Involvement in an Inequity Task with Dogs.
    Brucks D; Marshall-Pescini S; Essler JL; McGetrick J; Huber L; Range F
    Front Psychol; 2017; 8():270. PubMed ID: 28293204
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 63. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task.
    Bräuer J; Bös M; Call J; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Mar; 16(2):273-85. PubMed ID: 23090682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 64. Metacognition in canids: A comparison of dogs (Canis familiaris) and dingoes (Canis dingo).
    Royka AL; Johnston AM; Santos LR
    J Comp Psychol; 2020 Aug; 134(3):303-317. PubMed ID: 32804529
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 65. Differential effects of oxytocin on social sensitivity in two distinct breeds of dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Kovács K; Kis A; Pogány Á; Koller D; Topál J
    Psychoneuroendocrinology; 2016 Dec; 74():212-220. PubMed ID: 27665081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 66. Breed differences in social cognition, inhibitory control, and spatial problem-solving ability in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
    Junttila S; Valros A; Mäki K; Väätäjä H; Reunanen E; Tiira K
    Sci Rep; 2022 Dec; 12(1):22529. PubMed ID: 36581704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 67. Differences in persistence between dogs and wolves in an unsolvable task in the absence of humans.
    Rao A; Bernasconi L; Lazzaroni M; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F
    PeerJ; 2018; 6():e5944. PubMed ID: 30515358
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 68. Showing behavior in Animal Assisted Intervention and pet dogs.
    Cavalli C; Carballo F; Dzik MV; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2020 Oct; 179():104218. PubMed ID: 32791202
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 69. Artificially elevated oxytocin concentrations in pet dogs are associated with higher proximity-maintenance and gazing towards the owners.
    Pedretti G; Wirobski G; Range F; Marshall-Pescini S
    Physiol Behav; 2021 Aug; 237():113451. PubMed ID: 33974958
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 70. Observing animals and humans: dogs target their gaze to the biological information in natural scenes.
    Törnqvist H; Somppi S; Kujala MV; Vainio O
    PeerJ; 2020; 8():e10341. PubMed ID: 33362955
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 71. Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Soproni K; Miklósi A; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2001 Jun; 115(2):122-6. PubMed ID: 11459158
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 72. My owner, right or wrong: the effect of familiarity on the domestic dog's behavior in a food-choice task.
    Cook A; Arter J; Jacobs LF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):461-70. PubMed ID: 23995771
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 73. Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use of human social cues by the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) in an object choice task.
    Oliva JL; Rault JL; Appleton B; Lill A
    Anim Cogn; 2015 May; 18(3):767-75. PubMed ID: 25647172
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 74. The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs.
    Range F; Horn L; Viranyi Z; Huber L
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2009 Jan; 106(1):340-5. PubMed ID: 19064923
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 75. Trained dogs do not give up: Effects of advanced training on the persistence of domestic dogs.
    Cavalli C; Dzik MV; Brarda M; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2022 Nov; 203():104769. PubMed ID: 36283575
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 76. What are you or who are you? The emergence of social interaction between dog and an unidentified moving object (UMO).
    Gergely A; Petró E; Topál J; Miklósi Á
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(8):e72727. PubMed ID: 24015272
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 77. Marked referential communicative behaviours, but no differentiation of the "knowledge state" of humans in untrained pet dogs versus 1-year-old infants.
    Gaunet F; Massioui FE
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Sep; 17(5):1137-47. PubMed ID: 24676672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 78. Do pet dogs (
    Duranton C; Range F; Virányi Z
    R Soc Open Sci; 2017 Jul; 4(7):170349. PubMed ID: 28791164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 79. Sociability and gazing toward humans in dogs and wolves: Simple behaviors with broad implications.
    Bentosela M; Wynne CD; D'Orazio M; Elgier A; Udell MA
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2016 Jan; 105(1):68-75. PubMed ID: 26781052
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 80. Dogs and wolves differ in their response allocation to their owner/caregiver or food in a concurrent choice procedure.
    Isernia L; Wynne CDL; House L; Feuerbacher EN
    PeerJ; 2022; 10():e12834. PubMed ID: 35186459
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.