These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

132 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 35292871)

  • 21. Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task.
    Herrmann E; Melis AP; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Apr; 9(2):118-30. PubMed ID: 16395566
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Inferences about the location of food in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) in two sensory modalities.
    Sabbatini G; Visalberghi E
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):156-66. PubMed ID: 18489231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Responses of wild skuas (Catharacta antarctica ssp. lonnbergi) to human cues in cooperative and competitive social contexts.
    Danel S; Rebout N; Pinto L; Carette P; Bonadonna F; Biro D
    J Comp Psychol; 2023 Aug; 137(3):167-177. PubMed ID: 37639234
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Inhibitory control and cue relevance modulate chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) performance in a spatial foraging task.
    Völter CJ; Tinklenberg B; Call J; Seed AM
    J Comp Psychol; 2022 May; 136(2):105-120. PubMed ID: 35324222
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Behavioral cues that great apes use to forage for hidden food.
    Buttelmann D; Call J; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):117-28. PubMed ID: 17534674
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Inferential reasoning and modality dependent discrimination learning in olive baboons (Papio hamadryas anubis).
    Schmitt V; Fischer J
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Aug; 123(3):316-25. PubMed ID: 19685974
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Inference by exclusion in the red-tailed black cockatoo (Calyptorhynchus banksii).
    Subias L; Griffin AS; Guez D
    Integr Zool; 2019 Mar; 14(2):193-203. PubMed ID: 29316266
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible.
    Dwyer C; Cole MR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):442-448. PubMed ID: 30251105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Reasoning by inference: further studies on exclusion in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus).
    Pepperberg IM; Koepke A; Livingston P; Girard M; Hartsfield LA
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Aug; 127(3):272-81. PubMed ID: 23421751
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Domestic pigs' (Sus scrofa domestica) use of direct and indirect visual and auditory cues in an object choice task.
    Nawroth C; von Borell E
    Anim Cogn; 2015 May; 18(3):757-66. PubMed ID: 25650328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Seeing the experimenter influences the response to pointing cues in long-tailed macaques.
    Schmitt V; Schloegl C; Fischer J
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(3):e91348. PubMed ID: 24646501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Use of incidentally encoded memory from a single experience in cats.
    Takagi S; Tsuzuki M; Chijiiwa H; Arahori M; Watanabe A; Saito A; Fujita K
    Behav Processes; 2017 Aug; 141(Pt 3):267-272. PubMed ID: 28065774
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Owl monkeys (Aotus nigriceps and A. infulatus) follow routes instead of food-related cues during foraging in captivity.
    da Costa RS; Bicca-Marques JC
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(12):e115188. PubMed ID: 25517894
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Do dogs follow behavioral cues from an unreliable human?
    Takaoka A; Maeda T; Hori Y; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Mar; 18(2):475-83. PubMed ID: 25348065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Inferential reasoning abilities in wild-caught bumblebees.
    Martin-Ordas G
    Biol Lett; 2024 Jun; 20(6):20230561. PubMed ID: 38863346
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Inferences about the location of food in lemurs (Eulemur macaco and Eulemur fulvus): a comparison with apes and monkeys.
    Maille A; Roeder JJ
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Nov; 15(6):1075-83. PubMed ID: 22806271
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task?
    Kupán K; Miklósi Á; Gergely G; Topál J
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Mar; 14(2):259-68. PubMed ID: 21140185
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Inferential reasoning in the visual and auditory modalities by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus).
    Heimbauer LA; Johns TN; Weiss DJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 Nov; 133(4):496-501. PubMed ID: 31094534
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Association of food location with biological cues in the macaque monkey.
    Ishibashi H; Kumashiro M; Iriki A
    Keio J Med; 2004 Jun; 53(2):98-102. PubMed ID: 15247514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domestica) use human-given cues in an object choice task.
    Nawroth C; Ebersbach M; von Borell E
    Anim Cogn; 2014 May; 17(3):701-13. PubMed ID: 24197275
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.