These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

122 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 35842325)

  • 21. Determinants and outcomes of decision-making, group coordination and social interactions during a foraging experiment in a wild primate.
    Pyritz LW; Fichtel C; Huchard E; Kappeler PM
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(1):e53144. PubMed ID: 23326392
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Consistent Individual Differences Drive Collective Behavior and Group Functioning of Schooling Fish.
    Jolles JW; Boogert NJ; Sridhar VH; Couzin ID; Manica A
    Curr Biol; 2017 Sep; 27(18):2862-2868.e7. PubMed ID: 28889975
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Collective decision making by rational individuals.
    Mann RP
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2018 Oct; 115(44):E10387-E10396. PubMed ID: 30322917
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Methods for the effective study of collective behavior in a radial arm maze.
    Delcourt J; Miller NY; Couzin ID; Garnier S
    Behav Res Methods; 2018 Aug; 50(4):1673-1685. PubMed ID: 29464590
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Evolutionary stability of optimal foraging: partial preferences in the diet and patch models.
    Křivan V
    J Theor Biol; 2010 Dec; 267(4):486-94. PubMed ID: 20863839
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Democracy in animals: the evolution of shared group decisions.
    Conradt L; Roper TJ
    Proc Biol Sci; 2007 Sep; 274(1623):2317-26. PubMed ID: 17644505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Collective animal behavior from Bayesian estimation and probability matching.
    Pérez-Escudero A; de Polavieja GG
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2011 Nov; 7(11):e1002282. PubMed ID: 22125487
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Optimal construction of army ant living bridges.
    Graham JM; Kao AB; Wilhelm DA; Garnier S
    J Theor Biol; 2017 Dec; 435():184-198. PubMed ID: 28939347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Acute restraint stress alters food-foraging behavior in rats: Taking the easier Way while suffered.
    Tu BX; Wang LF; Zhong XL; Hu ZL; Cao WY; Cui YH; Li SJ; Zou GJ; Liu Y; Zhou SF; Zhang WJ; Su JZ; Yan XX; Li F; Li CQ
    Brain Res Bull; 2019 Jul; 149():184-193. PubMed ID: 31034944
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Anterior cingulate cortex involved in social food-foraging decision-making strategies of rats.
    Zhong X; Deng S; Ma W; Yang Y; Lu D; Cheng N; Chen D; Wang H; Zhang J; Li F; Li C; Huang HL; Li Z
    Brain Behav; 2017 Oct; 7(10):e00768. PubMed ID: 29075556
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Optimality of collective choices: a stochastic approach.
    Nicolis SC; Detrain C; Demolin D; Deneubourg JL
    Bull Math Biol; 2003 Sep; 65(5):795-808. PubMed ID: 12909251
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Evolution of heterogeneous perceptual limits and indifference in competitive foraging.
    Mann RP
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2021 Feb; 17(2):e1008734. PubMed ID: 33621223
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Adversity magnifies the importance of social information in decision-making.
    Pérez-Escudero A; de Polavieja GG
    J R Soc Interface; 2017 Nov; 14(136):. PubMed ID: 29187633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. A conceptual framework to predict social information use based on food ephemerality and individual resource requirements.
    Kohles JE; O'Mara MT; Dechmann DKN
    Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc; 2022 Dec; 97(6):2039-2056. PubMed ID: 35932159
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Maintaining homeostasis by decision-making.
    Korn CW; Bach DR
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2015 May; 11(5):e1004301. PubMed ID: 26024504
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. To follow or not? How animals in fusion-fission societies handle conflicting information during group decision-making.
    Merkle JA; Sigaud M; Fortin D
    Ecol Lett; 2015 Aug; 18(8):799-806. PubMed ID: 26013202
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. A multi-scale review of the dynamics of collective behaviour: from rapid responses to ontogeny and evolution.
    Ioannou CC; Laskowski KL
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2023 Apr; 378(1874):20220059. PubMed ID: 36802782
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Honeybee methodology, cognition, and theory: recording local directional decisions in interpatch foraging and interpreting their theoretical relevance.
    Najera DA; Jander R
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):251-63. PubMed ID: 21912853
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Foraging networks and social tolerance in a cooperatively breeding primate (Callithrix jacchus).
    De la Fuente MF; Sueur C; Garber PA; Bicca-Marques JC; Souto A; Schiel N
    J Anim Ecol; 2022 Jan; 91(1):138-153. PubMed ID: 34655252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Decision-making processes: the case of collective movements.
    Petit O; Bon R
    Behav Processes; 2010 Jul; 84(3):635-47. PubMed ID: 20435103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.