These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

131 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 36088669)

  • 41. Early conversational environment enables spontaneous belief attribution in deaf children.
    Meristo M; Strid K; Hjelmquist E
    Cognition; 2016 Dec; 157():139-145. PubMed ID: 27636329
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. [Theory of mind in Asperger syndrome].
    Duverger H; Da Fonseca D; Bailly D; Deruelle C
    Encephale; 2007 Sep; 33(4 Pt 1):592-7. PubMed ID: 18033148
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Neural correlates of belief- and desire-reasoning in 7- and 8-year-old children: an event-related potential study.
    Bowman LC; Liu D; Meltzoff AN; Wellman HM
    Dev Sci; 2012 Sep; 15(5):618-32. PubMed ID: 22925510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. An fMRI study on the comparison of different types of false belief reasoning: False belief-based emotion and behavior attribution.
    Döhnel K; Schuwerk T; Sodian B; Hajak G; Rupprecht R; Sommer M
    Soc Neurosci; 2017 Dec; 12(6):730-742. PubMed ID: 27705092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Modelling the executive components involved in processing false belief and mechanical/intentional sequences.
    Tsuji H; Mitchell P
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2019 Jun; 37(2):184-198. PubMed ID: 30198592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Mistaken max befriends Duplo girl: No difference between a standard and an acted-out false belief task.
    Priewasser B; Fowles F; Schweller K; Perner J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Mar; 191():104756. PubMed ID: 31865246
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Could, would, should: Theory of mind and deontic reasoning in Tongan children.
    Taumoepeau M; Kata 'F; Veikune 'H; Lotulelei S; Vea PT; Fonua '
    Child Dev; 2022 Sep; 93(5):1511-1526. PubMed ID: 35616232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. False belief attribution in children with Williams syndrome: the answer is in the emotion.
    Campos R; Martínez-Castilla P; Sotillo M
    J Intellect Disabil Res; 2017 Nov; 61(11):1003-1010. PubMed ID: 28809068
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. The unreliability of egocentric bias across self-other and memory-belief distinctions in the Sandbox Task.
    Samuel S; Legg EW; Lurz R; Clayton NS
    R Soc Open Sci; 2018 Nov; 5(11):181355. PubMed ID: 30564420
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday.
    Rubio-Fernández P; Geurts B
    Psychol Sci; 2013 Jan; 24(1):27-33. PubMed ID: 23175524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. The influence of group membership on false-belief attribution in preschool children.
    Witt S; Seehagen S; Zmyj N
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Oct; 222():105467. PubMed ID: 35667302
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Do I know what they know? Linking metacognition, theory of mind, and selective social learning.
    Dutemple E; Hakimi H; Poulin-Dubois D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Mar; 227():105572. PubMed ID: 36371850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Young children have difficulty ascribing true beliefs.
    Riggs KJ; Simpson A
    Dev Sci; 2005 May; 8(3):F27-30. PubMed ID: 15819751
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding.
    Phillips LH; Bull R; Allen R; Insch P; Burr K; Ogg W
    Cognition; 2011 Aug; 120(2):236-47. PubMed ID: 21624567
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Contamination in reasoning about false belief: an instance of realist bias in adults but not children.
    Mitchell P; Robinson EJ; Isaacs JE; Nye RM
    Cognition; 1996 Apr; 59(1):1-21. PubMed ID: 8857469
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age.
    Thoermer C; Sodian B; Vuori M; Perst H; Kristen S
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):172-87. PubMed ID: 22429040
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Why are bilinguals better than monolinguals at false-belief tasks?
    Rubio-Fernández P
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2017 Jun; 24(3):987-998. PubMed ID: 27572871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Relations between verb factivity and first-order and second-order false belief understanding: Evidence from Mandarin-speaking typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders.
    Li H; Leung MT
    Clin Linguist Phon; 2020; 34(1-2):185-200. PubMed ID: 31262203
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Belief attribution in deaf and hearing infants.
    Meristo M; Morgan G; Geraci A; Iozzi L; Hjelmquist E; Surian L; Siegal M
    Dev Sci; 2012 Sep; 15(5):633-40. PubMed ID: 22925511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. The cognitive benefits of bilingualism in autism spectrum disorder: Is theory of mind boosted and by which underlying factors?
    Peristeri E; Baldimtsi E; Vogelzang M; Tsimpli IM; Durrleman S
    Autism Res; 2021 Aug; 14(8):1695-1709. PubMed ID: 34008896
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.