These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

56 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 36151106)

  • 1. The origins of belief representation: monkeys fail to automatically represent others' beliefs.
    Martin A; Santos LR
    Cognition; 2014 Mar; 130(3):300-8. PubMed ID: 24374209
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Evidence against implicit belief processing in a blindfold task.
    Rothmaler K; Grosse Wiesmann C
    PLoS One; 2023; 18(11):e0294136. PubMed ID: 37956182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Flexible social monitoring as revealed by eye movements: Spontaneous mental state updating triggered by others' unexpected actions.
    Fogd D; Sebanz N; Kovács ÁM
    Cognition; 2024 Aug; 249():105812. PubMed ID: 38763072
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Attributing false beliefs about non-obvious properties at 18 months.
    Scott RM; Baillargeon R; Song HJ; Leslie AM
    Cogn Psychol; 2010 Dec; 61(4):366-95. PubMed ID: 21047625
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Infants Generalize Beliefs Across Individuals.
    Burnside K; Neumann C; Poulin-Dubois D
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():547680. PubMed ID: 33071864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires.
    Bartsch K; Wellman H
    Child Dev; 1989 Aug; 60(4):946-64. PubMed ID: 2758888
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Three- and 5-year-old children know their current belief might be wrong.
    Helming K; O'Madagain C; Tomasello M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2024 Jul; 246():106001. PubMed ID: 39032186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Belief reversals as phase transitions and economic fragility: a complexity theory of financial cycles with reflexive agents.
    Davis J
    Rev Evol Polit Econ; 2020; 1(1):67-84. PubMed ID: 38624356
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence in autism spectrum disorders.
    Senju A
    Neuroscientist; 2012 Apr; 18(2):108-13. PubMed ID: 21609942
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Spontaneous attribution of underspecified belief of social partners facilitates processing shared information.
    Hegedüs AM; Király I
    Sci Rep; 2022 Sep; 12(1):15892. PubMed ID: 36151106
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Can infants adopt underspecified contents into attributed beliefs? Representational prerequisites of theory of mind.
    Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Csibra G
    Cognition; 2021 Aug; 213():104640. PubMed ID: 33757642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Do non-human primates really represent others' ignorance? A test of the awareness relations hypothesis.
    Horschler DJ; Santos LR; MacLean EL
    Cognition; 2019 Sep; 190():72-80. PubMed ID: 31026672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. 14-Month-olds anticipate others' actions based on their belief about an object's identity.
    Buttelmann F; Kovács ÁM
    Infancy; 2019 Sep; 24(5):738-751. PubMed ID: 32677281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15.
    ; ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16.
    ; ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17.
    ; ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18.
    ; ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19.
    ; ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20.
    ; ; . PubMed ID:
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 3.