These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

106 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 36179930)

  • 21. Awake canine fMRI predicts dogs' preference for praise vs food.
    Cook PF; Prichard A; Spivak M; Berns GS
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2016 Dec; 11(12):1853-1862. PubMed ID: 27521302
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Use of incidentally encoded memory from a single experience in cats.
    Takagi S; Tsuzuki M; Chijiiwa H; Arahori M; Watanabe A; Saito A; Fujita K
    Behav Processes; 2017 Aug; 141(Pt 3):267-272. PubMed ID: 28065774
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Differential effects of oxytocin on social sensitivity in two distinct breeds of dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Kovács K; Kis A; Pogány Á; Koller D; Topál J
    Psychoneuroendocrinology; 2016 Dec; 74():212-220. PubMed ID: 27665081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Dogs' (Canis familiaris) attention to human perception: Influence of breed groups and life experiences.
    Heberlein MT; Turner DC; Manser MB
    J Comp Psychol; 2017 Feb; 131(1):19-29. PubMed ID: 28080077
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Dogs (Canis familiaris) evaluate humans on the basis of direct experiences only.
    Nitzschner M; Melis AP; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(10):e46880. PubMed ID: 23056507
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Neurobiological underpinnings of dogs' human-like social competence: How interactions between stress response systems and oxytocin mediate dogs' social skills.
    Buttner AP
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2016 Dec; 71():198-214. PubMed ID: 27593441
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Do more sociable dogs gaze longer to the human face than less sociable ones?
    Jakovcevic A; Mustaca A; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2012 Jun; 90(2):217-22. PubMed ID: 22342201
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Why do dogs look back at the human in an impossible task? Looking back behaviour may be over-interpreted.
    Lazzaroni M; Marshall-Pescini S; Manzenreiter H; Gosch S; Přibilová L; Darc L; McGetrick J; Range F
    Anim Cogn; 2020 May; 23(3):427-441. PubMed ID: 32090291
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Third-party social evaluations of humans by monkeys and dogs.
    Anderson JR; Bucher B; Chijiiwa H; Kuroshima H; Takimoto A; Fujita K
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2017 Nov; 82():95-109. PubMed ID: 28077260
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Learning and owner-stranger effects on interspecific communication in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 May; 81(1):44-9. PubMed ID: 19429195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Can dogs (Canis familiaris) detect human deception?
    Petter M; Musolino E; Roberts WA; Cole M
    Behav Processes; 2009 Oct; 82(2):109-18. PubMed ID: 19686949
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. An Investigation on Social Representations: Inanimate Agent Can Mislead Dogs (Canis familiaris) in a Food Choice Task.
    Abdai J; Gergely A; Petró E; Topál J; Miklósi Á
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(8):e0134575. PubMed ID: 26241747
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Reassessing shelter dogs' use of human communicative cues in the standard object-choice task.
    Osborne T; Mulcahy NJ
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213166. PubMed ID: 30845205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Dogs' Individual Sensitivities to Being Observed by Their Owners While Performing a Repetitive Fetching Task.
    Kiss O; Kis A; Scheiling K; Topál J
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():1461. PubMed ID: 32760316
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Dogs' recognition of human selfish and generous attitudes requires little but critical experience with people.
    Carballo F; Freidin E; Casanave EB; Bentosela M
    PLoS One; 2017; 12(10):e0185696. PubMed ID: 29045426
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Dogs and their human companions: the effect of familiarity on dog-human interactions.
    Kerepesi A; Dóka A; Miklósi Á
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():27-36. PubMed ID: 24548652
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks.
    Lazarowski L; Dorman DC
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():60-7. PubMed ID: 25245303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. I saw where you have been--The topography of human demonstration affects dogs' search patterns and perseverative errors.
    Péter A; Topál J; Miklósi Á; Pongrácz P
    Behav Processes; 2016 Apr; 125():51-62. PubMed ID: 26869220
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Soproni K; Miklósi A; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2001 Jun; 115(2):122-6. PubMed ID: 11459158
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Dogs Identify Agents in Third-Party Interactions on the Basis of the Observed Degree of Contingency.
    Tauzin T; Kovács K; Topál J
    Psychol Sci; 2016 Aug; 27(8):1061-8. PubMed ID: 27268590
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.